The Cost of Strategic Control: Attenuation of Adaptation Jordan A. Taylor, Azeen M. Ghorayshi, and Richard B. Ivry Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley When instructed, people can employ an explicit strategy to compensate for a visuomotor rotation. However, as training continues, participants make increasingly larger errors in the direction of the strategy (Mazzoni & Krakauer, 2006). This overcompensatory drift is due to an implicit motor learning process that uses an error signal based on the difference between the planned reach location and the observed visual feedback. This system appears to be isolated from the strategic plan, and with adaptation, overrides a successful strategy. Here we present a series of behavioral experiments that allow us to develop a statespace model to elucidate the interaction of strategic and implicit contributions to visuomotor adaptation. First we replicated the drift effect reported by Mazzoni and Krakauer (2006). Participants viewed a display of eight circles separated by 45°, and moved to a cued target. A counterclockwise visual rotation (-45°) was unexpectedly introduced, resulting in a substantial error (Fig. 1). Participants were then told to use a "corrective" strategy, aiming to the circle immediately clockwise to the cued target. This strategy initially resulted in minimal error; the cursor landed near the cued target. However, errors gradually increased in the direction of the strategy-defined target, such that by the end of the block, movement heading was about 57° from the cued target. This block was followed by a no-rotation block during which participants were told to stop using the strategy. An aftereffect provided a signature of implicit adaptation. We initially employed an output-disturbance, state-space model of motor learning (Eqs. 1-3). This model gradually compensates for a visuomotor rotation (Fig. 2, black). However, without a model of the explicit strategy, it cannot capture the rapid initial reduction in error or the overcompensatory drift. Therefore, we modeled the strategy as a setpoint/reference signal that fed directly into the output equation as well as the control equation. The model (Eqs. 4-5) predicts the initial reduction in errors and that participants will eventually overcompensate by 45° to reach the strategy setpoint (Fig. 2, cyan). The magnitude of the drift is attenuated when the strategy leakage gain (Ks) is less than 1 (Fig. 2, magenta). The strategy leakage in the setpoint model captures the observed drift and aftereffect (Fig. 1, fit is black line). The setpoint model predicts that participants will eventually overcompensate by 45°; to test this (unlikely) prediction, we extended the strategy phase to 320 trials in Experiment 2. Drift was always present over the first hundred movements. With more trials, the drift was reversed with participants reducing endpoint error (No-Reward group, Fig 3a). However, the washout block revealed large aftereffects, indicating that implicit adaptation remained robust. Two additional groups were tested to explore variables that we expected may attenuate the drift and reduce implicit learning. A “Reward” group received bonus points based on target accuracy. By emphasizing target accuracy, we hypothesized that participants would adjust their strategy to improve performance and thereby reduce drift. For a “No-Aiming Target” group, only the target circle was visible on each trial. We expected this would attenuate drift because the absence of the aiming target would eliminate a source of visual information used to generate the error signal for implicit adaptation. As expected, drift was attenuated for both groups (Figs. 3b-c). Moreover, the aftereffect was reduced (Fig. 3d), especially in the No-Aiming group. To account for the influence of reward, we modeled the strategy as a function of the expected reward (Sutton & Barto, 1990). This model (Eq. 6) provides an excellent fit (Fig. 4a). The model indicates that, compared to the Reward group, participants in the No-Reward group tolerated a larger error window (Fig. 4c). Interestingly, the No-Reward group was equally sensitive to reward but gave greater weighting to previous rewards (Fig. 4d). To account for the effect of eliminating the aiming target, we modeled the controller as a weighted combination of visual and prioprioceptive feedback relative to the strategy (Eq. 7; see Cheng & Sabes, 2007). Again, the model provided an excellent fit (Fig. 4a), with the parameters indicating that prevention of the direct comparison of visual error and aiming location dramatically reduced implicit learning (Fig. 4b,d). We find that the overcompensatory drift can be accounted for by a state-space learning model in which an embedded strategic setpoint varies as a function of expected reward. This mechanism allows for rapid improvements in performance during the initial stages of learning. However, a cost associated with utilizing this mechanism is a reduction in the magnitude of implicit learning.

B

C

(4) yn = xn + sn + drot (5) un = Kssn - Kyn

Setpoint Model

1. The state (x) is updated by the decaying memory (A) of the previous state and the sensitivity (B) to the feedback controller (u) 2. Target error (y) in movement (n) is a function of the implicit state (x) plus the contribution of visuomotor rotation (drot) 3. For simplicity, the controller (u) was modeled as output feedback, which is the error (y) multiplied by a negative gain (K). 4. Error (y) is the output of the strategy (s), implicit state (x), and the rotation (drot) 5. A strategy (s) multiplied by a strategy gain (Ks) and was incorporated into u, and s≡-drot for 41< n <121. D

Target Error

A

Disturbance Model

Aftereffect Aftereffect

30

30

25

25

20 15

Target Error

Figure 2: Model predictions. Disturbance model (black). Setpoint model (cyan). Setpoint with variable leakage gain (Ks, magenta). Rotation is present for 80 movements (white region), and absent before and after this phase (shaded region).

Figure 1. Replication experiment. The rotation was presented for 80 movements (white region). Participants experienced the rotation for the first two movements before being instructed to use the strategy (magenta circle). Setpoint model fit (black)

(1) xn+1 = Axn + Bun (2) yn = xn + drot (3) un = -Kyn

10

No−Reward Reward No−Aiming Targets

20 15 10 5 0

5

1

20

40

60

80

Movement Number

0

1

20

40

60

80

Movement Number

Figure 3. Experiment 2. Participants first practiced moving to the cued target without a rotation (black) and while using the strategy without a rotation (orange). Rotation block (white region). The rotation was experienced for the first 2 movements without using the strategy (Xs). A) No-Reward (rotation, blue; washout, cyan). B) Reward (rotation, red; washout, magenta) C) No-Aiming Targets (rotation, green; washout, lt. green). D) Aftereffects (binned) Strategy Reward Model (6) sn+1 = sn + δ(rn - wr(E{r(1:n-1)})) a. rn = e

2 −yn 2σ 2

Aiming Target Model (7) un = Kssn - K[wn vn + (1-wv)pn] a. vn ≡ yn b. pn ≡ yn - drot

6. The strategy (s) is updated by the previous strategy, weighted (δ) by the difference between the previous reward and weighted (wr) expectation (E) of reward from previous rewards. The reward (r) is a Gaussian function of the error (y) and the reward window is given by σ. 7. The control signal (u) is a weighted combination (wv) of vision (v) and proprioception (p) relative to the strategy setpoint (s). The visual feedback (v) is the same as the error (y) and the proprioceptive feedback (p) is the same as (y), but is not rotated. Equations 1, 4, 5 and 6 were used to model the differences between the No-Reward and Reward conditions. To model the differences between the Reward and No-Aiming Targets conditions, the parameters from Eq. 6 were held fixed and Eq. 7 replaced Eq. 5.

A B C D Figure 4. Model fits for the No-Reward (blue), Reward (red), and NoAiming Targets (green) conditions. B) Changes over time in the hidden implicit state (dashed) and strategy (solid). C) Estimated reward window (σ) for No-Reward group (blue) and Reward group (red), Shading is 95% confidence interval of the mean. D) Model parameters of interest between No-Reward (blue) and Reward (red) conditions: strategy leakage (Ks), reward sensitivity (δ), expectation of reward (wr). Weighting of visual error, relative to the strategy, (wv) was fit separately for Reward (red) and No-Aiming Targets (green).

The Cost of Strategic Control: Attenuation of ... - Princeton University

As expected, drift was attenuated for both groups (Figs. 3b-c). Moreover, the aftereffect was reduced (Fig. 3d), especially in the No-Aiming group. To account for the influence of reward, we modeled the strategy as a function of the expected reward. (Sutton & Barto, 1990). This model (Eq. 6) provides an excellent fit (Fig. 4a).

420KB Sizes 0 Downloads 194 Views

Recommend Documents

Attenuation of Adaptation - Princeton University
strategy, it cannot capture the rapid initial reduction in error or the overcompensatory drift. Therefore, we modeled the strategy as a setpoint/reference signal that ...

Sources of Wage Inequality - Princeton University
Jan 14, 2013 - strong empirical support. Helpman et al. ... facts that support the mechanism of firm$ ..... An International Comparison, Chicago: University of ...

Trade, Merchants, and the Lost Cities of the ... - Princeton University
Jun 27, 2017 - multiple ancient cities within their boundary. Using 2014 ..... The lower panel presents simple statistics (mean, minimum and maximum). 32 ...

Trade, Merchants, and the Lost Cities of the ... - Princeton University
Jun 27, 2017 - raphy, we conjecture that the locational advantage brought by natural ... records all come from merchants' archives, and primarily deal with business ...... Eaton, J. and S. Kortum (2002): “Technology, Geography and Trade,” ...

New Immigrants.qxd - Princeton University
Westminster Register Office. Standing beside a framed photo ..... satellite television, cheap phone calls and the internet, people in developing countries are more ...

Painting with Triangles - Princeton Graphics - Princeton University
By con- trast, programs like Adobe Illustrator and Inkscape let a user paint ... effect. These arbitrary polygons are costly to render however, and. “smooth” effects are only created via many .... Next the outer stroke polygons are rendered in 50

china and the world history of science - Princeton University
1. For over a century, Europeans have heralded the success of West- .... as the Zhejiang junks first built in 1699 for the Ningbo-Nagasaki trade between Japan ...

New Immigrants.qxd - Princeton University Press
2. Immigrants: Your Country Needs Them grey-haired man in a bright red, fur-trimmed robe ..... ing for a few years in the Valley and set up companies that trade.

net neutrality - cs.Princeton - Princeton University
Jul 6, 2006 - of traffic when your browser needs to fetch a new page from a server. If a network provider is using ... hand, applications like online gaming or Internet telephony (VoIP), which rely on steady streaming of interactive .... The VPN user

New Immigrants.qxd - Princeton University Press
months while learning English; a forty-six-year-old Romanian dental technician who described ..... remote locations to complete their degree courses online. And.

Nuts and Bolts of Encryption - cs.Princeton - Princeton University
Center for Information Technology Policy. Department of Computer Science ... device, with encryption protecting the data should a malicious party get access to the device. Encrypted communication​ allows ... Encryption on a device such as a smartph

When Losses Turn Into Loans: The Cost of ... - Harvard University
Jan 2, 2018 - A one euro change in credit supply leads firms to adjust their labor spending by 16 cents and their investment ... First, distance from the capital constraint may be correlated with unobserved bank quality. We address this problem by re

When Losses Turn Into Loans: The Cost of ... - Harvard University
Jan 2, 2018 - to map our cross-sectional firm-level regression results into aggregate productivity growth. Based on these partial equilibrium estimates, the EBA intervention accounts for over 50% of the decline in aggregate productivity in 2012. This

Chapter 2 [PDF] - Princeton University Press
enables us to apply the tools used in the analysis of stationary models to study economies with sustained ...... of computer hardware and software. Thus we may ...

Curse-of-Complexity Attenuation Using Semidefinite Programming ...
Curse-of-Complexity Attenuation Using Semidefinite Programming in. Solution ... namic programming equation reduces to a Hamilton-Jacobi- ..... the kth step. The algorithm keeps only the L(k) quadratics,. ̂ak i with the highest values according to th

Prior Expectations Bias Sensory ... - Princeton University
In a separate analysis, we estimated BOLD amplitudes for each single trial, using the .... weights, we take our training data Bloc and regress those onto our hypo-.

Vision Based Self-driving Car - Princeton University
The world is very complicated. • We don't know the exact model/mechanism between input and output. • Find an approximate (usually simplified) model between input and output through learning. • Principles of learning are “universal”. – soc

Chapter 2 [PDF] - Princeton University Press
For more information send email to: ... These economists published two pathbreaking articles in the same year, 1956 (Solow, 1956;. Swan .... will study in Chapter 8) is that technology is free: it is publicly available as a nonexcludable, ... Definit

Prior Expectations Bias Sensory ... - Princeton University
segment in a 360° circle (Fig. 1A) using two buttons of an MR-compatible button box to rotate the line clockwise or anticlockwise. The initial di- rection of the line ...

Exporting and Organizational Change - Princeton University
Jul 18, 2017 - The computations in this paper were done at a secure data center .... of management (or L + 1 layers of employees, given that we call the ... of length z costs ¯wcz (c teachers per unit of knowledge at cost ¯w per teacher).

Exporting and Organizational Change - Princeton University
Jul 18, 2017 - We study the effect of exporting on the organization of production within firms. .... their technology (and so the marginal product of labor is higher) or .... Learning how to solve problems in an interval of knowledge .... We use conf

When Human Rights Pressure is ... - Princeton University
Sep 9, 2016 - have access to a good VPN to access foreign websites (Time 2013). On the other side, Chinese ... Times 2015), which was debated on social media. .... comparison to AU criticism 19, while AU criticism itself has no effect20.

Contribution of stretch reflexes to locomotor control - University of Alberta
After recovery, deafferented animals show little change in yield of the ... drive learned over a period of time may replace the missing stretch reflex ..... one without sensory contribution (a–c), i.e., when stretch reflexes contribute 30% of overa