Cogn Process (2013) 14:205–208 DOI 10.1007/s10339-013-0545-5

SHORT REPORT

Spatial language and the psychological reality of schematization Kevin J. Holmes • Phillip Wolff

Received: 6 December 2012 / Accepted: 18 January 2013 / Published online: 13 February 2013 Ó Marta Olivetti Belardinelli and Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Abstract Although the representations underlying spatial language are often assumed to be schematic in nature, empirical evidence for a schematic format of representation is lacking. In this research, we investigate the psychological reality of such a format, using simulated motion during scene processing—previously linked to schematization—as a diagnostic. One group of participants wrote a verbal description of a scene and then completed a change detection task assessing simulated motion, while another group completed only the latter task. We expected that effects of simulated motion would be stronger following language use than not, and specifically following the use of spatial, relative to non-spatial, language. Both predictions were supported. Further, the effect of language was scene independent, suggesting that language may encourage a general mode of schematic construal. The study and its findings illustrate a novel approach to examining the perceptual properties of mental representations. Keywords Spatial language  Schematization  Mental simulation  Language–thought interface

This article is part of the special issue on ‘‘Spatial Learning and Reasoning Processes’’, guest-edited by Thomas F. Shipley, Dedre Gentner and Nora S. Newcombe. Handling editor of this manuscript: Thomas F. Shipley. K. J. Holmes (&) Department of Linguistics, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA e-mail: [email protected] P. Wolff Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA e-mail: [email protected]

Introduction What is the nature of the representations underlying word meanings? Some recent research suggests that these representations may be relatively detailed, given evidence that language comprehension engages sensorimotor processes (Zwaan and Madden 2005). Work in lexical semantics, in contrast, has long held that the representations underlying word meanings, especially spatial terms, are highly schematic (Landau and Jackendoff 1993; Talmy 1983). Describing an object’s location, for example, is said to involve conceptualizing the object as a ‘‘dimensionless speck’’ (Pinker 2007, p. 48) or a ‘‘simple lump or blob’’ (Landau and Jackendoff 1993, p. 228). The view from lexical semantics, while compelling, has not, however, been empirically tested. In the present research, we offer such a test. Specifically, we investigate whether words encoding spatial relations lead to the schematization of space. A method for examining the representations associated with spatial language is suggested by research on mental simulation. In a study by Freyd et al. (1988), participants viewed a series of schematic line drawings in which a pedestal supporting a plant suddenly disappeared. The main finding was that participants spontaneously simulated the effects of gravity, indicated by greater insensitivity to downward relative to upward changes in the plant’s position. More recently, Holmes and Wolff (2010, 2013) replicated Freyd et al.’s findings across a wider range of scenes, but found that simulated motion was much more pronounced for line drawings than photorealistic materials. This schematic advantage highlights simulated motion as a diagnostic of schematization. If spatial language is represented schematically, describing a scene using spatial language should be more likely to elicit simulated motion than describing the same scene using non-spatial language.

123

206

To test this possibility, we had one group of participants write a verbal description of a photorealistic scene, followed by a change detection task assessing simulated motion. Another group completed only the latter task. Given the relative coarseness with which word meanings encode experience in general (Gleitman and Papafragou 2005; Wolff and Malt 2010), we expected that simulated motion would be more pronounced following language use than not. We also expected, however, that simulated motion would be stronger specifically after the use of spatial, relative to non-spatial, language. Of additional interest was whether any effect of language use on simulated motion would depend on the particular scene described. If simulated motion effects for a given scene are observed only after describing the very same scene, they could be viewed as a kind of thinking for speaking (Slobin 1996), with language modulating simulation processes online. However, if such effects are not scene dependent, this would imply a more lingering influence of language on scene construal (Wolff and Holmes 2011). To address this issue, we examined simulated motion in participants who described the same scene used in the change detection task compared to those who described a different scene.

Cogn Process (2013) 14:205–208

Results As predicted, simulated motion was more pronounced following language use than not. A 2 (condition) 9 2 (position: up vs. down) ANOVA for accuracy yielded a main effect of position, F(1,61) = 5.20, p = .03, and an interaction, F(1,61) = 3.97, p = .05. Only participants in the language condition simulated downward motion, indicated by lower accuracy on down than up trials (see Fig. 1c).2 To examine whether simulated motion was stronger following spatial relative to non-spatial language use, we coded participants’ descriptions for spatial relational (e.g., against, supporting) and descriptive (e.g., leafy, marble) terms. The simulated motion effect in the language condition was positively correlated with the relative frequency of spatial relational (see Fig. 1d), but not descriptive, terms (p [ .3), indicating that participants who used more spatial language showed greater simulated motion.3 Finally, we compared simulated motion in participants who described the change detection scene versus a different scene. Simulated motion was evident in both groups [same: t(17) = 2.22, p = .04; different: t(17) = 2.16, p = .05], indicating that the effect of language was not scene dependent.

Method Discussion Sixty-three undergraduates participated for course credit or payment.1 In the language condition, participants spent 5 min writing a detailed description of the scene in Fig. 1a (N = 18) or Fig. 1b (N = 18) and then completed a change detection task. In this task, each trial began with the scene in Fig. 1a, showing a potted plant atop a pedestal. A blank screen appeared next, followed by a scene with the plant in the same position, but without the pedestal. Another blank screen preceded the final scene, which showed the plant in the same position, slightly raised, or slightly lowered (0.15 cm). Participants indicated whether the plant was in the same or different position compared to the previous scene by pressing one of two computer keys. Each display appeared for 250 ms, except the final scene, which remained onscreen until participants responded. There were 60 randomly ordered trials (20 for each plant position). In the no-language condition (N = 27), participants completed only the change detection task.

1

All participants were Emory University students. Eight additional participants were excluded for making one kind of response (either same or different) on more than 75 % of trials.

123

Using language to describe a scene, especially spatial language, led people to simulate motion during subsequent scene processing, but no such effect occurred in the absence of verbalization. Given previous research linking simulated motion to schematization (Holmes and Wolff 2010, 2013), these results suggest that the representations generated during language use may be relatively lacking in detail. Our findings are consistent with research showing that children are better able to align relational structures when presented with relatively sparse stimuli (Gentner and 2

An analogous ANOVA on correct reaction times (RTs) yielded main effects of condition, F(1,61) = 5.08, p = .03, and position, F(1,61) = 15.40, p \ .001, but no interaction, p [ .3. Slower responses occurred in the language condition (M = 1161 ms, SD = 282) compared to the no-language condition (M = 1002 ms, SD = 295), and when the target object was shifted down (M = 1160 ms, SD = 377) versus up (M = 1003 ms, SD = 260), suggesting that there was no speed-accuracy trade-off. Although the interaction did not reach significance, the asymmetry in sensitivity to downward versus upward changes was descriptively larger in the language condition (M = 202 ms; d = .68) compared to the nolanguage condition (M = 118 ms; d = .46). 3 The correlation was also positive, though not significantly so, r(34) = .16, p = .35, when the degree of simulated motion was defined in terms of accuracy (i.e., accuracy on up trials minus accuracy on down trials).

Cogn Process (2013) 14:205–208

207

Fig. 1 a, b Displays described in the language condition. c Accuracy was significantly lower on down than up trials in the language condition, t(35) = 3.14, p = .003, d = .53, but not the no-language condition, t(26) = .20, p [ .8 (error bars: ±1 SEM). d Significant

positive correlation between the relative frequency of spatial– relational terms in participants’ descriptions and the degree of simulated motion, r(34) = .34, p = .05

Rattermann 1991) or when exposed to spatial language (Loewenstein and Gentner 2005), but they go beyond such work in using simulated motion to gain insight into the perceptual properties of the representations supporting such abilities. Further, we observed simulated motion effects even for scenes different from those just described, implying that schematization is not the product of specific words, but rather spatial language more generally. We suggest that spatial language may encourage a particular mental set, leading people to construe experience schematically even after language use. How long such effects of language persist is an intriguing question for future research.

thought and language: interrelations in development. Cambridge University Press, London, pp 225–277 Gleitman L, Papafragou A (2005) Language and thought. In: Holyoak K, Morrison B (eds) Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 633–661 Holmes KJ, Wolff P (2010) Simulation from schematics: dorsal stream processing and the perception of implied motion. In: Catrambone R, Ohlsson S (eds) Proceedings of the 32nd annual conference of the cognitive science society. Cognitive Science Society, Austin, pp 2704–2709 Holmes KJ, Wolff P (2013) Schematic is dynamic: realism, construal, and simulated motion (in preparation) Landau B, Jackendoff R (1993) ‘‘What’’ and ‘‘where’’ in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behav Brain Sci 16:217–265 Loewenstein J, Gentner D (2005) Relational language and the development of relational mapping. Cogn Psychol 50:315–353 Pinker S (2007) The stuff of thought: language as a window into human nature. Penguin Books, New York Slobin DI (1996) From ‘‘thought and language’’ to ‘‘thinking for speaking’’. In: Gumperz J, Levinson SC (eds) Rethinking linguistic relativity. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 70–96 Talmy L (1983) How language structures space. In: Pick H, Acredolo L (eds) Spatial orientation: theory, research, and, application. Plenum Press, New York, pp 225–282 Wolff P, Holmes KJ (2011) Linguistic relativity. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 2:253–265

References Freyd JJ, Pantzer TM, Cheng JL (1988) Representing statics as forces in equilibrium. J Exp Psychol Gen 117:395–407 Gentner D, Rattermann MJ (1991) Language and the career of similarity. In: Gelman SA, Byrnes JP (eds) Perspectives on

123

208 Wolff P, Malt BC (2010) The language–thought interface: an introduction. In: Malt BC, Wolff P (eds) Words and the mind: how words capture human experience. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 3–15

123

Cogn Process (2013) 14:205–208 Zwaan RA, Madden CJ (2005) Embodied sentence comprehension. In: Pecher D, Zwaan R (eds) Grounding cognition: the role of perception and action in memory, language, and thought. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 224–245

Spatial language and the psychological reality of ... - Springer Link

Feb 13, 2013 - Abstract Although the representations underlying spatial language are often assumed to be schematic in nature, empirical evidence for a schematic format of representation is lacking. In this research, we investigate the psycholog- ical reality of such a format, using simulated motion during.

232KB Sizes 1 Downloads 243 Views

Recommend Documents

Nepali Concepts of Psychological Trauma: The Role of ... - Springer Link
Mar 23, 2010 - School of Human Evolution and Social Change, Arizona State University, ... culture reveals that PTSD was not a universal and ever-present way of framing ... severe hardship with great courage and strength, to a ''postwarrior'' class wh

Sparse spatial sampling for the computation of motion ... - Springer Link
Jan 10, 2006 - Abstract The avian retino-tecto-rotundal pathway plays a central role in motion analysis and features complex con- nectivity. Yet, the relation between the pathway's structural arrangement and motion computation has remained elusive. F

Golog Speaks the BDI Language - Springer Link
Department of Computer Science and Engineering. York University, .... used when assigning semantics to programs: the empty (terminating) program nil; the ..... “main BDI thread” is inspired from the server example application in [8]. ..... Benfie

Encoding of variability of landmark-based spatial ... - Springer Link
Feb 24, 2010 - Abstract Recent evidence suggests humans optimally weight visual and haptic information (i.e., in inverse pro- portion to their variances). A more recent proposal is that spatial information (i.e., distance and direction) may also adhe

Facilitation of learning spatial relations among ... - Springer Link
Sep 24, 2009 - Received: 20 July 2009 / Revised: 10 September 2009 / Accepted: 14 September 2009 / Published online: ... ronment or interactive 3-D computer-generated virtual ... learning spatial relations among locations by visual cues.

Evidence against integration of spatial maps in ... - Springer Link
Sep 3, 2008 - ORIGINAL PAPER. Evidence against integration of spatial maps in humans: generality across real and virtual environments. Bradley R. Sturz · Kent D. Bodily · JeVrey S. Katz ·. Debbie M. Kelly. Received: 28 March 2008 / Revised: 2 Augu

Spatial methods for plot-based sampling of wildlife ... - Springer Link
Sep 19, 2007 - The data are assumed to come from a spatial stochastic ..... Patterson HD, Thompson R (1971) Recovery of interblock information when block ...

Spatial methods for plot-based sampling of wildlife ... - Springer Link
Sep 19, 2007 - Monitoring ecological populations is an important goal for both ... units, call it τ(z), where z is a vector of the realized values of a spatial ..... The distance between sample units was computed in kilometers from the center.

Fine-scale spatial genetic structure and within ... - Springer Link
Fine-scale spatial genetic structure and within population male-biased gene-flow in the grasshopper. Mioscirtus wagneri. Joaquın Ortego • Maria Pilar Aguirre • Pedro J. Cordero. Received: 11 May 2010 / Accepted: 20 January 2011 / Published onlin

Spatial and temporal deforestation dynamics in ... - Springer Link
Spatial and temporal deforestation dynamics in protected and unprotected dry forests: a case study from Myanmar. (Burma). Melissa Songer Æ Myint Aung Æ Briony Senior Æ Ruth DeFries Æ. Peter Leimgruber. Received: 4 January 2008 / Accepted: 18 Sept

Evidence against integration of spatial maps in humans - Springer Link
Abstract A dynamic 3-D virtual environment was con- structed for humans as an open-field analogue of Blaisdell and Cook's (2005) pigeon foraging task to determine if humans, like pigeons, were capable of integrating separate spatial maps. Participant

eContractual choreography-language properties ... - Springer Link
we give the schema definition [25] and additional doc- umentation online .... environment, e.g., by workflow management systems. Fur- ...... file is a data package.

eContractual choreography-language properties ... - Springer Link
Full list of author information is available at the end of the article theories .... tion systems [39,40] to enable business collaboration and ... a so-called business-network model (BNM) [41] in the ...... IEEE Computer Society, Washington, DC, USA.

Augmented reality registration algorithm based on ... - Springer Link
CHEN Jing1∗, WANG YongTian1,2, GUO JunWei1, LIU Wei1, LIN JingDun1, ... 2School of Computer Science and Technology, Beijing Institute of Technology, Beijing 100081, China; .... One is degrees and the other is meters or ... these years many successf

Natural Language as the Basis for Meaning ... - Springer Link
practical applications usually adopt shallower lexical or lexical-syntactic ... representation, encourages the development of semantic formalisms like ours.

Natural Language as the Basis for Meaning ... - Springer Link
Our overall research goal is to explore how far we can get with such an in- ...... the acquisition of Kaltix and Sprinks by another growing company, Google”, into a .... invent, kill, know, leave, merge with, name as, quote, recover, reflect, tell,

Production and validation of the pharmacokinetics of a ... - Springer Link
Cloning the Ig variable domain of MAb MGR6. The V-genes of MAb MGR6 were reverse-transcribed, amplified and assembled to encode scFv fragments using the polymerase chain reaction essentially as described [6], but using the Recombi- nant Phage Antibod

Comparison of MINQUE and Simple Estimate of the ... - Springer Link
1,2Department of Applied Mathematics, Beijing Polytechnic University, Beijing ... and a Project of Science and Technology of Beijing Education Committee.

Comparison of MINQUE and Simple Estimate of the ... - Springer Link
Vol.19, No.1 (2003) 13–18. Comparison of MINQUE and Simple Estimate of the. Error Variance in the General Linear Models. Song-gui Wang. 1. , Mi-xia Wu. 2.

Comparison of MINQUE and Simple Estimate of the ... - Springer Link
of an estimate ̂θ of a scalar parameter θ is defined by MSE(̂θ) = E(̂θ − θ). 2 ..... panel data and longitudinal data, X and V may satisfy the condition MV M ...

On the psychological reality of verb typologies
Our specific processing predictions take into account previous findings from studies .... experiment was run on Apple Macintosh computers running PsyScope (Cohen et al. 1993). 6. Results ..... The developer intentionally destroyed the city. 6.

The Incredible Economics of Geoengineering - Springer Link
Dec 6, 2007 - As I shall explain in this paper, its future application seems more likely than not. ... because the incentives for countries to experiment with ...

The Strength of Weak Learnability - Springer Link
high probability, the hypothesis must be correct for all but an arbitrarily small ... be able to achieve arbitrarily high accuracy; a weak learning algorithm need only ...