9328 • The Journal of Neuroscience, September 17, 2008 • 28(38):9328 –9329

Journal Club Editor’s Note: These short, critical reviews of recent papers in the Journal, written exclusively by graduate students or postdoctoral fellows, are intended to summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. For more information on the format and purpose of the Journal Club, please see http://www.jneurosci.org/misc/ifa_features.shtml.

Sources of Spatial and Feature-Based Attention in the Human Brain Marius V. Peelen* and Ryan E. B. Mruczek* Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey 08540 Review of Egner et al. (http://www.jneurosci.org/cgi/content/full/28/24/6141)

Attention is a critical component of visual perception because it involves the allocation of limited processing resources depending on current task demands. Most research has focused on our ability to covertly attend to specific regions within the visual field (spatial attention), often ignoring our perhaps equally important ability to attend to specific visual features across the visual field (feature-based attention). The behavioral benefits of both spatial and feature-based attention are unmistakably clear during visual search, a task that we perform often in our daily lives. For example, when searching for an item in a room (e.g., a pencil), you may have prior knowledge of the item’s spatial location (e.g., on top of the desk) and one or more of its visual features (e.g., yellow). Both types of information are typically used to guide the deployment of attention and speed the search process. Converging evidence from neuropsychological, electrophysiological, neuroimaging, and neurostimulation studies points to a critical role of frontal and parietal regions in the top-down deployment of spatial attention (Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000). Much less is known about brain regions underlying featureReceived July 29, 2008; accepted July 30, 2008. *M.V.P. and R.E.B.M. contributed equally to this work. Correspondence should be addressed to Marius V. Peelen, Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Green Hall, Washington Road, Princeton, NJ 08540. E-mail: [email protected]. DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3562-08.2008 Copyright©2008SocietyforNeuroscience 0270-6474/08/289328-02$15.00/0

based attention, although several studies suggest that it may recruit a frontoparietal network similar to that involved in spatial attention (Liu et al., 2003). This raises the interesting possibility that spatial and feature-based attention involve common brain areas in frontoparietal cortex. To address this question, one would have to directly compare neural mechanisms of spatial and feature-based attention within the same subjects and using comparable tasks. With an appropriate design, such a study might also be able to address to what extent spatial and feature-based information are independently represented within these top-down source regions. In a study published recently in The Journal of Neuroscience, Egner et al. (2008) took a novel and elegant approach to address these questions using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in human subjects. Using a factorial design, spatial and feature-based information were independently manipulated in a parametric manner. On each trial, subjects were presented with a central cue followed by a search display consisting of a blue and red diamond presented on both the left and right sides of a central fixation point. One of these four diamonds was missing a corner; subjects were instructed to indicate whether the missing corner was on the top or bottom of this target diamond. The cue that preceded the search display could provide information about either the location of the target diamond (left or right of fixation; spatial information), the color of the target dia-

mond (red or blue; feature-based information), or both. Furthermore, the reliability of the two types of cues was independently varied in a parametric manner (50, 70, or 90% reliable). For example, the cue could indicate that the target was 70% likely to be on the left side of the display and 90% likely to be red. Their design thus allowed for an investigation of the neural mechanisms of both spatial and feature-based attention, as well as their interaction. Reaction time data indicated that subjects used both types of cues to locate the target, and more reliable cue information resulted in faster responses. Furthermore, the effects of spatial and feature-based information on reaction times were comparable in magnitude. Whole-brain group analyses (n ⫽ 14) of fMRI data, testing for the parametric effects of both types of cue information separately, showed that an increase in either spatial or feature cue information (which were manipulated orthogonally) was associated with increased activity in a network of frontal and parietal regions typically observed in attention studies; both types of cues activated the intraparietal sulcus, frontal eye fields, inferior frontal cortex, and medial frontal cortex. A subsequent conjunction analysis, testing for regions significantly involved in processing both cue types, confirmed the overlap of the activations observed in the two separate analyses; significant activation was again evident in the frontoparietal network, including the four regions

Peelen and Mruczek • Journal Club

listed above. These results provide evidence that spatial and feature-based attention are represented, at least in part, in common brain regions. It should be noted, however, that these results came exclusively from group-averaged spatially smoothed data (9 mm kernel width). This leaves open the possibility that anatomically distinct regions for spatial and feature-based attention exist at the level of single subjects. The large intersubject variability in the location of attentionrelated frontal and parietal regions [Serences and Boynton (2007), their supplemental Fig. 7] highlights this point. To further explore the relationship between spatial and feature-based attention, the authors focused on the regions activated in the conjunction analysis and compared responses across the nine cue conditions (3 spatial ⫻ 3 feature levels). Within these regions, there were significant linear effects of both spatial and feature-based information, confirming the previous analyses. Importantly, no interaction between the two types of cues was observed in any of the regions, suggesting that the effect of the spatial cues was independent from the information provided by the feature cues, and vice versa. Together, the authors argue, these findings suggest that spatial and featurebased information are independently represented in shared brain regions. Furthermore, and as conveyed by the title of their study, they argue that these findings provide evidence for an integrated representation of spatial and feature-based information in frontal and parietal regions. What is meant by “integrated” and “independent” representations of these two types of attention? Each voxel contains several thousand neurons whose individual contributions to the bloodoxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal cannot be directly assessed. Based on the results of the conjunction analysis, spatial and feature-based attention signals appear integrated at the gross anatomical level observable by the BOLD signal. At

J. Neurosci., September 17, 2008 • 28(38):9328 –9329 • 9329

the same time, the additivity of spatial and feature-based attention in these regions suggests that the representation of spatial and feature-based information may be represented independently of each other at the neural level. The current data are consistent with two alternative hypotheses regarding the representation of spatial and featurebased attention in the frontoparietal network. The first is that spatial and featurebased information are represented by a single neuronal population. On this account, neurons encode aspects of both spatial location (e.g., left) and the attended feature (e.g., red). Note that this model does not require “conjunction neurons,” which would be active only when attending to, e.g., a red object in the left visual field. Rather, selectivity for spatial location and a given feature may be represented orthogonally, such that spatial tuning and feature tuning bear no systematic relationship to one another across the population of neurons. The alternative hypothesis is that spatial and featurebased information are represented by closely spaced, perhaps interspersed, but ultimately separate neuronal populations. This organization would appear integrated at the level of the BOLD signal, but not at the level of single neurons. The authors note that single-unit recordings in monkeys will be useful in helping to differentiate between these two possibilities. Alternative imaging methods may also offer additional insights. In particular, fMRI-adaptation paradigms and multivoxel pattern analysis have been used successfully to indirectly dissociate closely spaced neuronal populations (Grill-Spector and Malach, 2001; Peelen and Downing, 2007). Additionally, using high-resolution fMRI and detailed individual-subject analyses may reveal a fine-grained organization not apparent at the group-average level (Schwarzlose et al., 2005). Each of these methods has its own associated disadvantages, but it is only through the convergence of evidence

from multiple techniques that we will fully understand the neural mechanisms of spatial and feature-based attention. The results of Egner et al. (2008) support the notion that the frontoparietal network might be a general-purpose attention control system, not specific to spatial tasks. Indeed, it seems unfeasible that each set of features that we can attend to would be represented in an anatomically distinct cortical region. However, spatial attention may be “special” in that visual space is a fundamental aspect of the visual world, whereas the specific features that are behaviorally relevant for guiding attention are often learned through experience. The study by Egner et al. (2008) provides an elegant approach to investigating the top-down contributions to spatial and feature-based attention signals. Variations on this paradigm, in conjunction with more advanced imaging and analysis methods, will further enhance our understanding of the basic mechanisms of spatial and feature-based attention in the human brain.

References Egner T, Monti JM, Trittschuh EH, Wieneke CA, Hirsch J, Mesulam MM (2008) Neural integration of top-down spatial and feature-based information in visual search. J Neurosci 28:6141– 6151. Grill-Spector K, Malach R (2001) fMR-adaptation: a tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. Acta Psychol (Amst) 107:293–321. Kastner S, Ungerleider LG (2000) Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 23:315–341. Liu T, Slotnick SD, Serences JT, Yantis S (2003) Cortical mechanisms of feature-based attentional control. Cereb Cortex 13:1334 –1343. Peelen MV, Downing PE (2007) Using multivoxel pattern analysis of fMRI data to interpret overlapping functional activations. Trends Cogn Sci 11:4 –5. Schwarzlose RF, Baker CI, Kanwisher N (2005) Separate face and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus. J Neurosci 25:11055–11059. Serences JT, Boynton GM (2007) Feature-based attentional modulations in the absence of direct visual stimulation. Neuron 55:301–312.

Sources of Spatial and Feature-Based Attention in the ...

Sep 17, 2008 - tion (e.g., on top of the desk) and one or more of its visual features (e.g., yellow). Both types of information are typically used to guide the deployment of attention and speed the search process. Converging evidence from neuropsy- chological, electrophysiological, neuro- imaging, and neurostimulation ...

55KB Sizes 1 Downloads 215 Views

Recommend Documents

Selective attention to spatial and non-spatial visual ...
and the old age group on the degree to which they would be sensitive to .... Stimulus presentation was controlled by a personal computer, running an ...... and Brain Sciences 21, 152. Eason ... Hartley, A.A., Kieley, J., Mckenzie, C.R.M., 1992.

Improved selective and divided spatial attention in early ...
Feb 7, 2006 - sighted individuals in sensory sensitivity tasks, simple reaction time task, and selective and divided attention tasks. (see Fig. 1) with individually ...

capturing multisensory spatial attention - NeuroBiography
closed-circuit television camera and monitor was installed in front of the ...... (1997) used neutral objects (e.g., ping pong balls or a square of cardboard), ...

On the spatial extent of attention in object-based visual ...
(ANOVA)ofthe median RT data, with the factors ofcon- dition (3 levels) and ... sponses recorded, using an IBM-PC-compatible computer at- tached to a VGA ... 0.2 em in diameter and was located as for the center ofthe dash that it replaced.

On the spatial extent of attention in object-based visual ...
1981; Hoffman et aI., 1983) as with any object-based limit. Baylis and Driver (1993) devised a way to compare one- object and two-object judgments for identical ...

Scope of attention, control of attention, and intelligence ...
that variance was shared between scope and control, and the rest was unique to one or the other. Scope and control of ... ward an object appearing on the screen, an “antisaccade” movement ...... service of memory that was poorer in children.

the use and adaptation of feng shui in spatial ... - MOBILPASAR.COM
the harmful effects with the technology and knowledge we have nowadays, it could be why the reason Feng ... Figure 1:Feng Shui Analysis (Smith, 2006, p.10) ...

spatial and non spatial data in gis pdf
spatial and non spatial data in gis pdf. spatial and non spatial data in gis pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu.

Attention Reverses the Effect of Prediction in Silencing ...
Nov 2, 2011 - In order to drive subjects to allocate all ... infrared eye tracking system in the scanner (Sensomotoric Instruments, ..... This explanation is hard to.

fluorosis in children and sources of fluoride around lake elementaita ...
schools available in the area, and the homesteads of non-school-going children. All male and female ..... The rural water fluoride project: Technical report. Inter-.

The role of attention in illusory conjunctions - Springer Link
a major source of support for the feature-integration the- ory since, unlike other ... either within the attended subset or outside it, but not be- tween the attended and ...... Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21, 247-250. POSNER, M. 1. (1980).

Survey of Noise Sources in Bulk CMOS
The noise behavior of bulk CMOS devices is dominated primarily by two noise sources: thermal noise and flicker (1/f) noise. Other sources that are sometimes ...

fluorosis in children and sources of fluoride around ... - Semantic Scholar
exposure. There have been indications that uptake of fluoride from other sources like food, dust and beverages may be many times higher than that of water.1,2.

The role of attention in illusory conjunctions
man & Souther, 1986) proposes that features of objects are initially ...... FANG, S. P., & Wu, D. (1989). illusory conjunctions in the perception of Chinese ...

Financial Development and the Sources of Growth and ...
†Cologne Graduate School in Management, Economics and Social Sciences, University of Cologne, Germany. ... and talent across production units. .... a further advantage is that we can still view the system on two dimensions by working with.

Financial Development and the Sources of Growth and ...
the worldwide technology frontiers in 1965 and 2005, thereby providing the efficiency ... education of the adult population in country i at time t and define labor in ...... 171–293. GOLDSMITH, R. W., Financial Structure and Development (New ...

Sources of Health Insurance and Characteristics of the Uninsured ...
believe in the business case for providing health benefits today, but in the future they may ..... Figure 20, Reasons Workers Chose Not to Participate in Own Employer's Health Plan, Wage and Salary Workers Ages 18–64,. 2005 ...... technology.

Sources of Health Insurance and Characteristics of the Uninsured ...
Sep 22, 2011 - Uninsured: Analysis of the March 2011 Current Population ...... to offer them if a compensation package comprised of both wages and health.

Sources of Health Insurance and Characteristics of the Uninsured ...
trends are due to job losses resulting from the recent recession and slow economic ... to health benefits in the work place, and coupled with uncertainty about the ...

Disentangling the Sources of Pro&social Behavior in ...
What motivates workers on their job? For certain ... data entry job on two separate occasions (one hour each). On the first ..... ton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Sources of Health Insurance and Characteristics of the Uninsured ...
Sep 22, 2011 - In contrast, 45.3 percent of blacks had coverage and 39.2 percent of Hispanics had it. Even after controlling for poverty status, whites were ...

Sources of Health Insurance and Characteristics of the Uninsured ...
Paul Fronstin is director of the Health Research and Education Program at the ...... provided by the Centers for Medicare & Medicaid Services (CMS), the federal ...

Sources of Health Insurance and Characteristics of the Uninsured ...
Sep 22, 2011 - when the data s to health be of the opulatio er and percent. U.S. Census B in coverage fo is insured. tage of the no. 0. Increases in onelderly indiv e was 18.5 ...... function, and nonprofit status of this organization and the exempt