Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

1

1

Reproductive Skew in Vertebrates (eds. Hager and Jones)

2 3 4

The Causes and Consequences of Reproductive Skew in Male Primates

5 6

Nobuyuki Kutsukake (1, 2) and Charles L Nunn (3, 4)

7 8 9

(1) Department of Biological Sciences, Graduate School of Sciences, The University of Tokyo

10

(2) Laboratory for Biolinguistics, RIKEN Brain Science Institute

11

(3) Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany

12

(4) Department of Integrative Biology, University of California, Berkeley

13 14 15 16

Address: Nobuyuki Kutsukake - Laboratory for Biolinguistics, Brain Science Institute, RIKEN, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako-shi, Saitama, 351-0198, JAPAN Tel: +81-48-462-1111-6823, fax: +81-48-467-7503

17 18 19

E-mail: [email protected]

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

20

2

INTRODUCTION

21

Reproductive skew theory attempts to explain the uneven distribution of

22

reproductive success among same-sexed group members by multiple social, ecological,

23

and genetic factors (Fig. 1; reviewed in Johnstone 2000). Reproductive skew theory has

24

often been divided into two broad categories known as transactional and compromise

25

frameworks.

26

make. In a version of the transactional framework known as the concession model, the

27

dominant individual controls the reproduction of subordinates and allows them to

28

reproduce in return for the subordinate staying in the group (i.e., the dominant offers a

29

“staying incentive”; Vehrencamp 1983a, b; Keller and Reeve 1994; Clutton-Brock

30

1998; Johnstone 2000). Retaining the subordinate is assumed to increase group

31

productivity (i.e., total reproductive output of a group) and fitness benefits of a

32

dominant, relative to the alternative of the subordinate leaving the group. In contrast, the

33

tug-of-war model, which is part of the compromise framework, suggests that the

34

dominant individual is unable to control the reproduction of subordinates completely

35

(Reeve et al. 1998; Cant 1998; Clutton-Brock 1998); the division of reproduction is

36

therefore determined by competition between a dominant and subordinate (Reeve et al.

37

1998; Cant 1998; Clutton-Brock 1998), which is assumed to decrease group

38

productivity. These models can be expanded into systems with more than two

39

individuals competing for reproduction (Johnstone et al 1999; Reeve and Emlen 2000),

40

including queuing systems (i.e., a subordinate acquiring a higher dominance position in

41

the future: Kokko & Johnstone, 1999; Ragsdale 1999; Mesterton-Gibbons et al. 2006).

42

These frameworks differ according to the assumptions that each of them

In this chapter, we consider the causes and the consequences of skew in male

43

primates.

44

frameworks into single conceptual models (Johnstone 2000; Reeve and Shen 2006), the

Although recent research has synthesized the transactional and compromise

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

3

45

classic dichotomy of the transactional (concession model) and compromise frameworks

46

(tug-of-war model) provides a useful starting point for investigating reproductive skew

47

in primates and will therefore be used here.

48

social groups.

49

to which reproduction or matings are skewed (Cowlishaw and Dunbar 1991; Bulger

50

1993; Kutsukake and Nunn 2006). Although inter-individual variation in male

51

reproductive success has been a central topic in primate research (e.g., Cowlishaw &

52

Dunbar 1991; Bulger 1993; Alberts et al. 2003; van Noordwijk and van Schaik, 2004),

53

only recently have researchers applied the theoretical frameworks of reproductive skew

54

to investigate patterns of mating and reproduction in male primates (Hager 2003;

55

Widdig et al. 2004; Bradley et al. 2005; Kutsukake and Nunn 2006).

Social primates live in relatively stable

In these groups, males can exhibit considerable variation in the degree

56

Figure 1 provides an overview of the topics covered in this chapter. First we

57

focus on the causes of skew, starting with an explanation of the POA model and how

58

this model corresponds to the newer theoretical frameworks for understanding

59

reproductive skew.

60

predictions of the tug-of-war and concession models, we review four case studies that

61

have explicitly introduced and used paternity data to investigate predictions of skew

62

models in primates, and we discuss a new research direction to examine predictions

63

from skew theory using phylogenetic comparative methods (Kutsukake and Nunn

64

2006).

In this first section, we also discuss the assumptions and

65

In the second part of this chapter, we discuss another new research direction:

66

investigating the consequences of reproductive skew on other biological traits (Fig. 1).

67

We focus on two examples.

68

within groups, and the other considers how patterns of skew might influence the spread

69

of sexually transmitted diseases. We conclude by identifying several areas for future

The first involves the effects of skew on relatedness

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

70

4

research, including comparative studies.

71 72

THE CAUSES OF REPRODUCTIVE SKEW

73

The priority-of-access (POA) model

74

The POA model (Altmann, 1962) has been the most influential framework

75

used to explain variation in reproduction among male primates (Altmann, 1962;

76

Altmann et al. 1996; Boesch et al. 2006). The model predicts that the dominant male

77

monopolizes reproduction within a group. However, the degree to which the dominant

78

male succeeds in this goal is affected by the number of oestrous females in the group.

79

When two or more females are in oestrus at the same time, the dominant male is unable

80

to mate guard all of them effectively, thus providing an opportunity for subordinate

81

males to mate. The model therefore makes predictions for the distribution of matings

82

within groups, with the dominant male obtaining the largest share, and subordinates

83

obtaining lesser amounts in proportion to their ranks.

84

Empirical studies provide evidence for the dominant male’s advantages in both

85

mating (Cowlishaw and Dunbar, 1991; Bulger, 1993; Ellis 1995; Alberts et al., 2003;

86

Kutsukake and Nunn 2006) and paternity success (van Noordwijk and van Schaik 2004).

87

In addition, some studies have investigated the effect of oestrous synchrony on the

88

distribution of matings, reproductive success and the number of males in a group (e.g.,

89

Bulger, 1993; Paul 1997; Nunn 1999a; Soltis et al. 2001; Takahashi 2004; van

90

Noordwijk and van Schaik 2004; Boesch et al 2006; Alberts et al 2006). In general,

91

these studies have shown that when more females are in oestrus, the ability of a

92

dominant male to control access to females is more limited. The effect of oestrous

93

synchrony has also been demonstrated in studies of non-primates (e.g., domestic cats,

94

Felis catus: Say et al. 2001).

5

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

95

The POA model has contributed greatly to primate research, but studies on

96

primates have produced variable results (Dunbar 1988; Cowlishaw and Dunbar 1991;

97

Bulger 1993; van Noordwijk and van Schaik 2004; Kutsukake and Nunn 2006).

98

some cases, researchers have uncovered the biological reasons for departures from the

99

POA model. For example, mate choice by females also can impact the distribution of

100

reproduction in ways that differ from predictions of POA (Dunbar 1988; Soltis 2004).

101

Females may confuse paternity by mating promiscuously and concealing ovulation –

102

both of which should decrease skew – or females may increase skew by copulating with

103

the dominant male during periods in which the probability of fertilization is high (van

104

Schaik et al. 1999, 2000; Nunn 1999b; van Noordwijk and van Schaik 2004).

In

105

Some researchers have incorporated the effect of the number of males in

106

evaluating the POA model (e.g., Alberts et al. 2003; 2006; Boesch et al. 2006), based on

107

the reasoning that it should be more difficult for a dominant male to monopolize

108

females when there are more males in the group who are competing for females

109

(Cowlishaw and Dunbar 1991). Our comparative work – discussed below – provides

110

evidence for this effect in analyses that control for phylogeny, suggesting that male

111

number is the primary factor that affects skew in social primates.

112

of males was not explicitly considered by Altmann (1962), here we call this framework

113

the extended-POA model, in order to separate it from the original POA model.

Because the number

114

The POA and skew models are not fundamentally different in their goals of

115

explaining the distribution of reproduction within groups. Relative to the POA model,

116

however, the reproductive skew framework provides a richer set of variables to consider,

117

potentially explaining more variation in male mating success. For example, skew

118

models take into account the possibilities for males to leave the group and either attempt

119

to breed on their own or join another group where their fitness would be greater, and

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

6

120

thus also the need for dominant males to provide staying incentives. The concession

121

model from the transactional framework also makes explicit assumptions about the

122

degree of control that dominant males have over reproduction, with the tug-of-war

123

models explicitly challenging the assumption of the dominant’s complete control of

124

subordinate reproduction. Skew models make use of data on relationships among

125

males, with greater skew predicted under the concession model when males are more

126

closely related. In addition to male reproductive success, they can be applied to

127

investigate female reproductive success.

128 129

Testing the reproductive skew frameworks

130

Evaluating whether a particular skew model applies to a species requires

131

information on multiple parameters. Quantification of these parameters is difficult in

132

any species, including primates.

133

studies of skew, such as in Hymenoptera, are difficult or unethical to attempt in primates,

134

in part because most primates have long lifespans and many are highly threatened. Here

135

we discuss two approaches: first to investigate the assumptions of different skew models

136

(Johnstone 2000; Magrath et al 2004), and second to test specific predictions in

137

observational and comparative studies.

Moreover, experiments common in other empirical

138 139

Testing assumptions of different skew models

140

The first assumption of the transactional framework is that the presence of

141

subordinates increases productivity and the fitness benefits of the dominant individual.

142

Positive relationships between male number and group productivity (or efficiency of

143

defense against extra-group males) have been reported in male primates (Wrangham

144

1999; Treves 2001). In wild chimpanzees, for example, intergroup aggression is mainly

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

7

145

conducted by males (Wrangham 1999), and the number of offspring and probability of

146

infant survival increases with the number of males (Boesch et al. 2006). This pattern

147

could occur through the combined effects of attracting females to the group and better

148

defense of the territory or offspring.

149

documented that a decrease in the number of males in a small group, possibly caused by

150

intergroup killing by the larger neighboring group, resulted in the transfer of females to

151

the larger group (Nishida et al., 1985). Thus, this assumption that subordinates provide

152

fitness benefits and higher group productivity could be met in species where males

153

defend a territory or a group of females, and in other situations in which dominant

154

males benefit from membership in multimale groups.

In another population, at Mahale, researchers

155

A key assumption of the concession model within the transactional framework

156

is that the dominant individual has complete control over reproduction by subordinates.

157

Field studies provide weak support for this assumption. In most species of social

158

primates, for example, the presence of a dominant individual does not suppress the

159

reproductive states of subordinates (see Carlson and Young, this volume), and complete

160

control must be difficult if there are too many rivals in a group. A dominant male can

161

often interrupt mating by subordinates, but in many cases he is ineffective in completely

162

preventing copulations by subordinate males (Soltis 2004). Various studies have further

163

shown that the degree to which the alpha male succeeds in reproduction decreases as the

164

number of rivals increases (van Noordwijk and van Schaik 2004). Finally, complete

165

control should be especially difficult in species living in fission-fusion societies

166

(Dunbar 1988), where subdivision of the group into foraging parties should make it

167

more difficult for males to monitor mating attempts by other males. These species

168

include chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus) and spider monkeys

169

(Ateles spp.).

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

8

170

An important assumption of the compromise framework is that group

171

productivity decreases as a result of competition between the dominant and subordinate.

172

Infanticide by males is widely observed in primates (van Schaik and Janson 2001) and

173

reduces group productivity. Correlational studies showed that groups with multiple

174

males are less productive than single-male groups (black-and-white colobus, Colobus

175

guereza: Dunbar 1987; Hanuman langurs, Semnopithecus entellus: Srivastava and

176

Dunbar, 1996), although the behavioral mechanism for how the presence of multiple

177

males affects male-male competition – and ultimately group productivity – is largely

178

unknown. Moreover, some studies reported positive effects of subordinate males on

179

group productivity (red howler monkeys, Alouatta seniculus: Crockett & Janson 2000;

180

mountain gorillas, Gorilla gorilla: Watts 2000). These results suggest that the links

181

between the number of subordinate males in a group and competition among males or

182

group productivity is not universal. Future studies should test this assumption more

183

broadly across species, including species living in multimale groups.

184

This brief summary suggests that males are unlikely to have complete control

185

over reproduction (as assumed in the concession model), and that group productivity

186

can either increase or decrease with the number of males and the intensity of

187

competitions among dominant and subordinate males (as predicted by transactional and

188

compromise frameworks, respectively).

189

appropriate for some species but not others, and quantitative testing of the assumptions

190

could help to disentangle which models should be investigated in different species.

191

Other models (and extensions of these models, such as social queuing or models that

192

incorporate multiple individuals) make additional assumptions (Kokko & Johnstone,

193

1999; Johnstone et al 1999; Reeve and Emlen 2000) that would be worth investigating

194

as skew frameworks are applied to primate mating systems.

Thus, a particular skew model could be

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

9

195 196

Testing specific predictions of skew models

197

Reproductive skew models also make different predictions for the effects of

198

demographic variables (number of males and females), female reproductive traits

199

(oestrous synchrony), and relatedness among males on patterns of reproductive skew

200

(Table 1). The tug-of-war model and the extended POA model predict that skew

201

decreases as the number of males in a group increases, based on the reasoning that it

202

will be difficult for a dominant male to control or monitor reproductive attempts by

203

other males when more rivals are present (Cowlishaw and Dunbar 1991; van Noordwijk

204

and van Schaik 2004). Similarly, increases in the number of females in a group should

205

decrease skew if this provides more mating opportunities for subordinate males

206

(Altmann 1962; Cowlishaw and Dunbar 1991; Bulger 1993; van Noordwijk and van

207

Schaik 2004).

208

Another prediction from the tug-of-war model and the POA model involve

209

female oestrous overlap (Table 1). Increased oestrous overlap, which results from a

210

long mating season, a long oestrous duration, or socially mediated synchrony, should

211

make it more difficult for a dominant male to monopolize a receptive female, thus

212

decreasing skew among males (Ridley 1986; Cowlishaw and Dunbar 1991; Paul 1997;

213

Shuster and Wade 2003).

214

causing dominant males to guard females over only part of their cycles (Packer 1979;

215

Bercovitch 1983; Alberts et al. 1996). Few mathematical or empirical models of

216

reproductive skew among males have considered the influence of female reproduction

217

and behavior; exceptions include the female control model, developed by Cant and

218

Reeve (2002), and studies of acorn woodpeckers (Melanerpes formicivorus, Haydock

219

and Koenig 2002), brown jays (Cyanocorax morio, Williams 2004), and some studies of

Similar effects can arise if the costs of guarding are high,

10

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

220

primates (e.g., Soltis et al. 2001; Charpentier et al 2005a; Boesch et al. 2006).

221

of these exceptions involve female effects on male monopolization, and therefore

222

address assumptions of the tug-of-war model. Because the primate socioecological

223

model focuses explicitly on female reproductive strategies as influencing male

224

behaviour (Dunbar 1988; Nunn 1999a; van Schaik et al. 1999), this is an area where

225

primatology has the potential to contribute to further development of skew models.

226

Many

Finally, the tug-of-war model predicts no relationship between male

227

relatedness and skew (Table 1).

228

circumstances in which males exert weaker control over close relatives (Reeve et al.

229

1998). In some circumstances, for example, dominants could increase their inclusive

230

fitness by exerting fewer restrictions on mating by related subordinates, thus generating

231

a negative association between relatedness and skew.

232

This relationship could even be negative in

The concession model predicts no association between demographic factors or

233

oestrous synchrony and skew (Table 1).

234

will impact patterns of skew, with high relatedness associated with high skew, due to the

235

expectation that related subordinate males can receive their “staying incentive” in the

236

form of inclusive fitness benefits (Keller and Reeve 1994; Johnstone 2000).

Instead, this model predicts that relatedness

237 238

Case studies of the causes of reproductive skew

239

Data on patterns of reproductive success are steadily growing in primates (van

240

Noordwijk and van Schaik 2004), offering potential for investigating whether

241

compromise or transactional frameworks are more appropriate for studying skew in

242

male primates (Widdig et al. 2004; Setchell et al. 2005; Charpentier et al. 2005a;

243

Bradley et al. 2005; Boesch et al. 2006).

244

framework appears to offer a better fit for primate males, and the extended POA model

As reviewed below, the compromise

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

11

245

may be equally powerful in explaining patterns of reproductive skew among male

246

primates. Even so, we should not let this blind us to the possibility that transactional

247

frameworks could account for additional variation in male skew, particularly when

248

males defend territories, as this is one way that group productivity can increase with the

249

number of males (see above).

250

In what follows, we review four case studies of male skew in

251

multimale-multifemale primate groups (Table 2).

252

exhaustive; rather we use selected examples to reveal how skew theory provides new

253

insights to variation in male reproductive success in primates.

254

section with a summary, and then present comparative evidence in the following

255

section.

These examples are not meant to be

We conclude this

256 257

Rhesus macaques

258

In rhesus macaques, males disperse from their natal groups, while females

259

remain in the group in which they were born. Widdig et al (2004) investigated

260

reproductive skew in a population on Cayo Santiago and found that the top-sire fathered

261

between 19 and 30% of the offspring per year over a six-year period, while 69 to 79%

262

of males sired no infants at all. In terms of specific tests, the authors showed that (1)

263

males exhibited significant variation in skew, with a measure of skew (the B-index,

264

Nonacs 2000) significantly different from zero in most tests; (2) the B index was not

265

significantly associated with either average pairwise relatedness among males nor

266

female synchrony (estimated indirectly from births); (3) heterozygosity of MHC genes

267

predicted male reproductive success, highlighting the potential role of female choice.

268

The authors concluded that their results support the compromise framework, as the

269

concession model would predict few effects of female choice, a lower level of

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

12

270

relatedness among breeders, and stronger control of group reproduction by resident

271

(dominant) males.

272 273 274

Mandrill In the wild, mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx) live in groups of up to several

275

hundred individuals (Abernethy et al 2002). Behaviour in these groups has not been

276

investigated, largely due to the difficulties of habituating and observing behavior of

277

mandrills in their natural habitat. Important information on this species has been

278

provided by research from a semi-free-ranging captive colony (CIRMF Mandrill

279

Colony, Gabon). In this population, only the alpha male exhibits the distinctive

280

secondary sexual traits (e.g., bright colour of the face) characteristic of this extremely

281

sexually dimorphic species. Although multiple males are present in the colony,

282

paternity analyses have shown that the alpha male fathers 69% of offspring, indicating

283

extreme reproductive skew among males (Setchell et al 2005).

284

Two studies have investigated different aspects of reproductive skew in this

285

colony. Although these authors studied the same groups, some results differed between

286

the studies, in part due to differences in the specific aims of each study, in samples

287

collected and variables that were analyzed, and in statistical approaches. In one of these

288

studies, Setchell et al (2005) investigated deviations in the alpha male’s reproductive

289

success from the expected value based on the POA model. The authors showed that

290

departures from the POA model increased as the number of males in a group increased

291

(Table 2), which fits predictions from the extended POA and the tug-of-war models. By

292

comparison, Charpentier et al. (2005a) studied factors affecting the failure of alpha

293

males to sire offspring.

294

oestrous synchrony, relatedness between the dominant male and females, and the

These authors reported that relatedness among males, female

13

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

295

number of males affected paternity of the dominant male. Specifically, (1) the dominant

296

male’s reproduction decreased as relatedness among males increased; (2) oestrous

297

overlap decreased reproduction by the dominant male; (3) relatedness between the

298

dominant male and females negatively affected reproduction by the alpha male.

299

Although the behavioural mechanism for this result is unknown, incest avoidance may

300

have played a role because the degree of heterozygosity correlated positively with

301

individual reproductive success (Charpentier et al. 2005b). (4) Counter to predictions

302

from the tug of war model and patterns found by Setchell et al. (2005) and more

303

generally in primates (van Noordwijk and van Schaik 2004; Kutsukake and Nunn 2006),

304

the number of males correlated positively with the proportion of offspring that the alpha

305

male sires (Charpentier et al, 2005a). To explain this result, Charpentier et al. (2005a)

306

suggested that competition among subordinates increased as the number of males

307

increases, deflecting competition away from the dominant male.

308

Although the effect of male number differed between the studies, both Setchell

309

et al. (2005) and Charpentier et al (2005a) concluded that the limited control model best

310

characterized this species; predictions of the concession model were never supported.

311

Setchell et al. (2005) also noted that conditions in wild mandrills might produce weaker

312

patterns of control than those found in the colony studied by these authors.

313 314

Mountain gorilla

315

There is variation in the number of males in groups of mountain gorillas

316

(Gorilla gorilla) in the Virunga mountains, with multimale groups representing 40% of

317

the groups in the population (Robbins et al 2001). Female reproductive cycles are short

318

and it is rare that the receptive periods of two or more females overlap.

319

overlap should tend to enable the dominant male to monopolize reproduction within a

The lack of

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

14

320

group. However, paternity analyses have shown that subordinates also reproduce to

321

some extent (about 15%), suggesting that the reproductive skew (estimated by the B

322

index) is high but not complete (Bradley et al 2005).

323

In another study, Robbins and Robbins (2005) used an individual-based

324

simulation model with demographic parameters from the same population studied by

325

Bradley et al. (2005) to investigate the expected reproductive success of subordinates

326

that remain in their group.

327

subordinate more than dispersing. However, the model revealed that the dominant

328

does not benefit from retention of subordinates, suggesting that dominant males do not

329

concede reproduction. Thus, both Bradley et al. (2005) and Robbins and Robbins (2005)

330

concluded that reproductive skew in this population corresponds better to predictions

331

from the tug-of-war model than the concession model.

The model revealed that remaining in a group benefits a

332 333

Chimpanzees

334

In chimpanzees, males remain in their natal group and exhibit a high degree of

335

a fission-fusion sociality. Females develop sexual swellings when they are in oestrus,

336

with synchronous oestrous relatively common. The dominant male has higher

337

reproductive success, but subordinate males also reproduce (Constable et al. 2001).

338

Boesch et al (2006) investigated paternity in chimpanzees of Taï National Park in Cote

339

d’Ivoire using long-term records.

340

the alpha male decreased as the number of males increased and when female oestrous

341

overlap increased.

342

extended POA model and the tug-of-war model.

These results therefore agree with predictions from both the

343 344

They found that the proportion of reproduction by

Summary of Case Studies

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

15

345

Overall, these studies suggest that limited control is a characteristic of male

346

behaviour in primates (Table 2) and that the tug-of-war model or the extended POA

347

model can explain variation in skew among male primates. However, these studies do

348

not completely reject the concession model for the following reasons. First, empirical

349

studies mainly tested predictions from mathematical models that were designed for

350

systems other than primates, often assuming that the group contains only two

351

individuals – a dominant and a subordinate. In contrast, mathematical models that

352

incorporate more realistic parameters, such as three or more group members or the

353

possibility of social queuing by subordinates, predict a reduced necessity of offering

354

incentives by a dominant individual to a subordinate (in particular to a unrelated

355

subordinate) relative to the two-player models (Kokko & Johnstone, 1999; Ragsdale

356

1999; Johnstone et al 1999; Reeve and Emlen 2000; Reeve and Shen, 2006). This makes

357

it difficult to draw firm predictions for how relatedness should correlate with patterns of

358

skew. Second, no studies in primates have succeeded in accurately quantifying

359

parameters that are necessary to test the skew model, in large part because it is difficult

360

to conduct experimental manipulations in primates. Crucially, these parameters include

361

the probability of solitary reproduction by subordinates, fitness benefit of the dominant

362

male when there are no subordinate males, and the effects of subordinate males’

363

presence on group productivity. Finally, the concession and tug-of-war models are not

364

mutually exclusive, and can in fact coexist within a single framework (Johnstone 2000;

365

Reeve and Shen, 2006).

366 367

Phylogenetic comparative analyses

368

Skew models have been regarded as a unifying framework for understanding

369

the diversity of social systems seen in animals (Keller and Reeve 1994; Sherman et al.

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

16

370

1995), but surprisingly few studies have examined broad evolutionary patterns of skew

371

within one clade of either vertebrates or invertebrates (Boomsma and Sundström 1998;

372

Duffy et al. 2000; Faulkes et al. 1997). Such comparative perspectives are important in

373

reproductive skew research for at least four reasons.

374

a means to understand the factors generating broad evolutionary patterns of skew

375

(Nonacs 2000) and therefore can assess the generality of a pattern across species,

376

leading to greater unification of models of social evolution. Second, comparative

377

approaches offer an opportunity to test assumptions and predictions of skew models

378

from an evolutionary perspective, and they can be used to test assumptions or

379

predictions of skew models. Third, by identifying differences among species,

380

comparative results can point to new variables to investigate in future field or laboratory

381

research. Finally, comparative research can be used to generate new hypotheses, which

382

can then be tested in the field or laboratory, or refined through theoretical models.

First, comparative studies provide

383

In previous primate research, cross-species comparisons have been conducted

384

to examine the effects of seasonality on variance in mating or reproductive success

385

(Cowlishaw and Dunbar 1991; Paul 1997). We conducted a phylogenetic comparative

386

analysis on the determinants of “mating” skew in male primates, based on a database of

387

species in multimale primate groups (in total from 84 studies representing 31 species in

388

17 genera, Kutsukake and Nunn 2006). Since few studies have investigated the

389

distribution of paternity for a sufficient number of primate species, we investigated

390

mating distribution. While many studies have shown that mating frequency predicts

391

reproductive success (e.g., Smith 1981; Pope 1990; Ohsawa et al. 1993; de Ruiter et al.

392

1994; Paul and Kuester 1996; Soltis et al. 1997; Alberts et al. 2006), other studies failed

393

to find such links (e.g., Curie-Cohen et al. 1983; Shively and Smith 1985; Inoue et al.

394

1991, 1993), possibly because many matings in primates are likely to be

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

17

395

non-reproductive (Soltis 2004). To deal with this problem, we used data that are most

396

tightly linked to male reproductive success whenever possible; specifically, we

397

preferred data on ejaculation frequency more than copulation frequencies, and

398

copulation data at times when conception was most likely to take place (Kutsukake and

399

Nunn 2006). Genetic information on actual reproduction in groups would clarify these

400

issues and allow skew to be examined more directly, but such data are not yet

401

sufficiently available to test the predictions in a comparative context.

402

In quantifying the magnitude of mating skew, we focus here on results using

403

the “maximum mating proportion” (Bulger 1993), which is the proportion of mating by

404

the most successful male.

405

(Nonacs, 2000) and lambda (Kokko and Lindström, 1997). We investigated the effects

406

of three variables: demographic factors (the number of males or females in a group),

407

female reproductive factors that are related to the difficulties of monopolizing oestrous

408

females (i.e., duration of the breeding season, duration of oestrus, and measures of

409

oestrous overlap), and male dispersal pattern (categorized as male philopatry or male

410

dispersal). Regarding male dispersal pattern, the concession model predicts high skew

411

in male philopatric species relative to species in which males disperse because there is

412

(1) a high probability that a dominant male has a brother within a group and (2) a lower

413

probability that subordinates will disperse. Taken together, these factors reduce the

414

need for the dominant male to provide a staying incentive.

We also examined other skew indices, including the B index

415

The main results of our study (Kutsukake and Nunn 2006) can be summarized

416

as follows. First, based on Nonac’s B, mating was significantly skewed among males in

417

75.4% of cases (43 / 57 cases), and the alpha male or resident male tended to mate more

418

frequently. Second, using the independent contrasts method (Felsenstein 1985) and

419

stepwise multiple regression, we found that only male number correlated with mating

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

18

420

skew (P<0.001), with the proportion of mating by the most successful male falling as

421

the number of males in a group increases (Fig. 2). Finally, neither female reproductive

422

proxies nor male dispersal pattern affected mating skew. Overall, these results are most

423

consistent with the tug-of-war model and partially consistent with the extended POA

424

model (in the sense that the number of males negatively affected skew).

425

This result raises the possibility that the effects of oestrous synchrony are not

426

universal to all primate species, its effects are weak, or synchrony is difficult to quantify,

427

all of which would limit our ability to detect a significant association in comparative

428

analyses given existing data. Although the intensity of the correlation between

429

dominance rank and reproductive success was affected by seasonality (Paul 1997), up to

430

now, few studies have investigated paternity among males in relation to oestrous

431

synchrony (Setchell et al 2005; Charpentier et al, 2005a; Boesch et al 2006).

432

One could argue that the concession model also predicts that mating skew

433

should decrease as the number of males increases, specifically if the dominant male

434

needs to pay staying incentives to each subordinate male. However, we also found a

435

similar negative relationship in an intraspecific analysis of wild chimpanzees

436

(Kutsukake and Nunn 2006). The negative relationship is not expected in a male

437

philopatric species, such as the chimpanzee, because subordinate males have few

438

opportunities for reproduction outside of their natal communities, and therefore do not

439

need an incentive to stay.

440

Even with this intraspecific analysis, however, we cannot firmly reject the

441

concession model.

442

and mating skew can also be explained by the concession model because reproductive

443

skew may decrease when the power difference between a dominant and subordinate is

444

small (e.g., in a group with many males; Cowlishaw and Dunbar 1991); therefore, the

For example, a negative relationship between the number of males

19

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

445

dominant may concede the reproduction as a ‘peaceful’ incentive to avoid a risky fight

446

with powerful rival males (Reeve and Ratnieks 1993). Indeed, the power differences

447

may be smaller in a group with a large number of males because one would expect that

448

males are, on average, more similar in age (and therefore competitive ability). This idea

449

needs further testing, but this example highlights the difficulty of testing between the

450

different skew models, even in well-studied mammalian species.

451

In addition, our comparative study does not reject the possibility that the

452

concession model applies to particular primate species, even if it is not a general

453

explanation for patterns of skew across primates. As is shown by a recent synthetic

454

model, the transactional framework and compromise framework are not mutually

455

exclusive (Johnstone 2000; Reeve and Shen 2006). So, one model may fit one species

456

but not others, or in certain demographic or ecological situations but not in others within

457

a species. For example, even within a species, the dominant male may be able to exert

458

complete control in a small group in which there is only one subordinate, but not in a

459

large group with multiple subordinates. This possibility can be tested by investigating

460

how the effects of relatedness on reproductive skew vary according to the number of

461

subordinate males in groups.

462

Although our approach focused on males in short time intervals, such as a

463

single breeding season, this approach can be used to examine complex life history

464

trajectories (patterns of lifetime reproductive success).

465

could be applied to both sexes.

466

primates can be estimated using long-term data. Finally, it would be interesting to apply

467

this approach to other clades in which data on reproduction and phylogeny are widely

468

available, such as birds, social insects, and in other well-studied mammalian groups,

469

such as rodents, ungulates and carnivores.

In addition, this approach

For example, reproductive success among female

20

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

470 471

Applying comparative approaches to other biological systems

472

Comparative tests can focus on either the predictions or the assumptions of

473

skew models, and testing is possible if researchers have quantitative data for the

474

distribution of reproduction or mating among group males.

475

stimulate further comparative research in other clades of animals, we list several

476

methodological practices for conducting comparative tests of predictions related to

477

reproductive skew models.

Here, in an attempt to

478

1) Carefully choose the hypotheses, predictions or assumptions to be tested.

479

Within the framework of the models and the biology of the organisms, the researcher

480

needs to consider alternative explanations and how different parameters might influence

481

the predictions of a skew model.

482

that are specific to the study animals because some parameters are difficult to quantify

483

in some clades.

It is also important to incorporate the characteristics

484

2) Collect data on mating or reproductive skew and other important variables

485

such as group composition (e.g., number of males and females), relatedness, female

486

behaviors, and reproductive biology. Data on reproduction are available in many

487

non-primates (e.g., Ellis 1995), which could be used for comparative analyses. It is also

488

important to obtain a phylogeny for the group of species being studied. “Supertrees”

489

and other large-scale, dated phylogenies are now available for many species

490

(Bininda-Emonds 2004), making this process easier than in the past.

491

3) Quantify the distribution of reproduction using several skew proxies

492

(Nonacs 2003). Many studies will not provide these measures directly, and may not

493

even provide information for the comparative biologist to calculate the measures.

494

Thus, it might be necessary to use a simple index that maximizes sample size (in terms

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

495

21

of the number of species).

496

4) Test the hypotheses using phylogenetic comparative methods, such as

497

independent contrasts (Felsenstein 1985; Harvey and Pagel 1991; Nunn and Barton

498

2001).

499

and Garland 2002), to test the statistical and evolutionary assumptions, and to determine

500

whether the results are robust to alternative assumptions.

It is important to check whether the data show phylogenetic signal (Blomberg

501 502

CONSEQUENCES OF REPORDUCTIVE SKEW

503

Previous studies mainly investigated the causes of the skew and tested specific

504

models. An important new direction in skew research is to consider the consequences of

505

reproductive skew on other biological traits, including social structure and individual

506

social strategies (Fig. 1; Heinze 1995; Widdig et al. 2001; Cant & English 2006). For

507

example, in some systems, the number of breeders and characteristics of the breeding

508

queue could influence optimal group size (Cant and English 2006). With the goal of

509

developing new questions for future studies, we briefly discuss two consequences of

510

reproductive skew in male primates: effects on within-group relatedness and the spread

511

of disease.

512 513

Reproductive skew and within-group relatedness

514

In species characterized by high skew, infants born in a short period are more

515

likely to be paternally related. For example, Widdig et al. (2004) found that in a

516

high-skew rhesus macaque troop at Cayo Santiago, 74% of the infants had at least one

517

paternal sibling in the group, and individuals had almost four times as many paternal as

518

maternal siblings.

519

fathered by different males, thus tending to reduce the level of relatedness at the group

In contrast, infants in low skew societies are more likely to be

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

22

520

level. The paternal relatedness among group members should have a major impact on a

521

wide range of social behaviours, including affiliation, cooperation, competition and

522

mate choice (Hamilton 1964, Chapais and Berman, 2003).

523

Several studies have suggested that individuals recognize paternal relationships

524

and adjust their behaviour accordingly. For example, skew is high in male western

525

gorillas, and the silverbacks of different groups are closely related (Bradley et al. 2004).

526

This result may explain the occurrence of non-agonistic encounters between groups

527

observed in this species, which might be unexpected in such a sexually dimorphic

528

species in which male-male competition is likely to be especially intense. Paternal

529

half-siblings are more affiliative with one another than unrelated individuals in rhesus

530

macaques (Widdig et al. 2001) and in savanna baboons (Smith et al. 2003; see also Silk

531

et al. 2006). Also in baboons, paternal half-siblings showed less affiliative and sexual

532

behaviour during consortships than did unrelated pairs (Alberts 1999). As a final

533

example, infants were supported by a biological father (Buchan et al. 2003) or were not

534

the target of infanticide by the biological father in species living in multimale groups

535

(Borris et al 1999a,b; Soltis et al. 2000).

536

When reproductive skew is high and the dominant male’s tenure is long enough

537

for his female offspring to mature sexually, it could be adaptive for the dominant male

538

to discriminate the paternity of the offspring and avoid mating with his daughters. In

539

wild white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus), for example, the probability of

540

reproduction by the alpha male varied with whether or not a female was a daughter of

541

the alpha male, with a lower probability of reproduction between the alpha male and his

542

daughter (Muniz et al. 2006). It would be interesting to investigate whether such incest

543

avoidance mechanisms are common in primates, because some studies have found

544

evidence for incest avoidance (Table 2), while others have not (Constable et al. 2001). If

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

23

545

incest avoidance is an important selective force, strong skew combined with long male

546

tenures could reduce future opportunity for the alpha male to reproduce within a group,

547

thus creating an incentive for secondary dispersal.

548

As discussed above, the degree to which paternal relatedness affects individual

549

behaviour and social structure represent important areas for future research. In addition,

550

it will be important to uncover the proximate mechanisms responsible for identifying

551

paternal kin (Rendall 2004).

552

of their information on the monopolization of receptive females as a proximate cue to

553

assess the probability that they are fathers of the offspring. Similarly, for human

554

observers, it may be possible to estimate the magnitude of reproductive skew a

555

posteriori from the genetic relatedness among infants and juveniles in a group.

It is possible, for example, that dominant males make use

556 557

Reproductive skew and the spread of infectious disease

558

Reproductive skew also can have consequences for patterns of social contact

559

within social units, thus impacting the spread of disease within primate groups (Nunn

560

and Altizer 2006).

561

example, one or a few males will gain access to the vast majority of mating

562

opportunities.

563

fight to improve or maintain their dominance ranks. This fighting causes wounds for

564

males by biting and scratching and can result in the spread of disease, as demonstrated

565

in the case of retroviruses (SIV and STLV) in a semi-free-ranging colony of mandrills

566

(Nerrienet et al. 1998). In addition to being involved in male intrasexual competition,

567

a high-ranking male in a high skew society also has better access to mates, resulting in

568

higher rates of sexual contact.

569

sexually transmitted diseases (STDs; Graves and Duvall 1995), potentially even

In a high skew primate group under the tug-of-war model, for

Thus, there is likely to be intense competition among males as they

Thus, such a male can act as a contact point for

24

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

570

selecting for reduced skew (Thrall et al. 2000; Kokko et al. 2002). If a female is

571

already infected with an STD at the time that a new male rises in rank, this male is

572

likely to become infected shortly after he attains high rank; he can thus serve as the

573

source of infection for the many females that he mates with during his tenure as the

574

alpha male.

575

A number of models have investigated the epidemiology of STDs in both human

576

(Anderson and May 1991) and non-human systems (Thrall et al. 1997; Boots and Knell

577

2002; Kokko et al. 2002).

578

Thrall et al. (2000) developed an individual based model to explore how variance in

579

mating success, patterns of female dispersal and mortality rates of both sexes influence

580

the spread of STDs.

581

males and females, every male would have one female if there was no skew (equivalent

582

to monogamy); each additional female assigned to a male means one less female for

583

another male, resulting in increased reproductive skew.

584

the prevalence of STDs is higher as the degree of polygyny (reproductive skew)

585

increases.

In the context of variance in male mating skew, for example,

Given that the simulated population had an equal number of

The simulations revealed that

586

A challenge in applying these concepts to generate testable predictions is that

587

low skew in multimale-multifemale primates groups can also favour the spread of an

588

STD.

589

higher rate of mating with more males throughout the female’s cycle, possibly as a

590

strategy to reduce the risk of infanticide (Hrdy and Whitten 1987; van Schaik et al.

591

1999).

592

(Anderson and May 1991).

593

even higher levels than revealed by models of STD spread under skew, such as the

594

Thrall et al. (2000) study, especially if most subordinates have some mating success.

Thus, if males have relatively equal access to females, this could result in a

And of course, increased promiscuity should increase the spread of an STD This promiscuity is likely to increase the prevalence to

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

25

595

Thrall et al.’s (2000) STD model provides a way out of this conundrum, however,

596

because output from the model also predicts a higher prevalence of infection in females

597

than in males as reproductive skew increases, i.e., a sex difference is predicted. Kokko

598

et al. (2002), in a different modeling approach, confirmed that female choice for a

599

particular (presumably high-ranking) male can also lead to higher prevalence of

600

infection in females.

601

only high prevalence (relative to, say, monogamy); increasing skew should also produce

602

a sex difference in the prevalence of an STD, with higher prevalence in females than in

603

males.

604

(Nunn and Altizer 2004).

605

correlate with skew and other variables that influence the establishment of an STD,

606

including mortality rates, dispersal, and differences in transmission probabilities

607

between the sexes (e.g., with females potentially being more susceptible to an STD).

608

In addition, it will be important to bring queuing or life history (age dependency) into

609

the STD models, because if most individual males have some mating opportunities over

610

their lifetimes, the difference in STD exposure between the sexes may become more

611

narrow.

Thus, a critical prediction is that higher skew will produce not

This prediction has been tested and supported using data on STDs in primates A next step is to examine whether sex-differences also

612 613

CONCLUSIONS

614

This chapter discussed the causes and consequences of reproductive skew in

615

male primates. Several studies have investigated the assumptions of the transactional

616

framework in primates in order to test skew theory. Empirical studies showed that the

617

tug-of-war model may explain the pattern of skew among males better than the

618

concession model. Our comparative studies revealed a negative association between the

619

number of males in a group and skew, which agrees with previous findings in primates

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

26

620

(Setchell et al. 2005; Boesch et al. 2006; reviewed in van Noordwijk and van Schaik

621

2004) and also agrees with predictions from the tug-of-war model. Therefore, we

622

tentatively conclude that incomplete control is a general characteristic of male primates,

623

but more studies are needed to test the assumptions or predictions of the concession

624

model.

625

The priority-of-access (POA) model (Altmann 1962) has had a major impact in

626

studies of male reproductive success in primates.

627

oestrous overlap on the distribution of reproduction among males in multi-male

628

multi-female groups, including non-primates. A major conclusion of our chapter is

629

that the POA model – especially an extended version that incorporates the number of

630

males – is almost indistinguishable from the compromise framework.

631

particularly true with regard to the predictions, where only one prediction differs (Table

632

1).

633

bottle.” This would be misleading, however, as the skew framework is actually much

634

broader than the previous POA model. For example, it builds significantly on POA by

635

encapsulating factors involving relatedness, breeding opportunities and costs of

636

dispersal.

This model highlighted the effect of

This is

It might therefore seem that the skew framework represents “new wine in an old

637

Several challenges remain for the future. First, the present mathematical

638

models of reproductive skew are not designed to apply to primate social systems. In

639

particular, it would be worthwhile to develop skew models that incorporate three or

640

more players (Johnstone et al 1999; Reeve and Emlen 2000), social queuing (Kokko &

641

Johnstone, 1999; Ragsdale 1999; Mesterton-Gibbons et al. 2006), female influences

642

such as incest avoidance (Cant & Reeve, 2002; Johnstone, 2000), and female choice for

643

males with particular biological traits (“good genes” or high dominance rank). Recent

644

mathematical models in which one individual adjusts behaviour in response to the

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

27

645

behaviour of the other individual (negotiation game: McNamara et al. 1999; Cant and

646

Shen 2006) may be more appropriate in primates, because social interactions in

647

primates change temporally according to the strategy of opponents. Also, an

648

individual-based model based on empirical demographic parameters would be a useful

649

tool for generating more refined predictions for patterns of skew in primates (e.g.,

650

Robbins and Robbins 2005).

651

Second, no empirical studies of primates have successfully estimated the

652

parameters that are needed to distinguish among the different skew models. These

653

parameters include the links between competition within groups and group productivity

654

and ecological constraint that determines the probability of solitary reproduction. This

655

may represent a limitation of skew theory, with very few predictions distinguishing the

656

different models.

657

composition, would help to more formally test skew theory in primates.

658

could be conducted in semi-free-ranging groups.

Nonetheless, experimental studies, including manipulating group Such tests

659

Third, most of the studies in primates estimate skew in a relatively short time

660

period. Thus, it is unknown how short-term skew is associated with long-term (i.e.,

661

lifespan) reproductive success (Altmann et al. 1996).

662

The consequences of reproductive skew have been largely unexplored, yet

663

these topics offer great opportunities for future research in primates. Irrespective of

664

causes of skew, how a given magnitude of skew affects social structure, individual

665

decision-making, and other biological traits that relate to reproduction is a promising

666

area for both empirical and theoretical research. For example, investigating the

667

relationship between skew and the prevalence of STDs could have important

668

implications for conservation biology, given that STDs often cause sterility (Canfield et

669

al. 1991; Lockhart et al. 1996).

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

28

670

In conclusion, bringing the skew paradigm to primatology may yield new

671

perspectives for understanding primate behaviour, specifically by integrating more

672

diverse factors that are relevant to male and female decisions on group formation,

673

interactions within groups, and reproductive strategies. Thus, skew models could play a

674

major role in developing an integrative model of primate socioecology. Key future

675

directions will involve developing skew models that are more appropriate for primates,

676

collecting data to test the assumptions and predictions of these models, and

677

investigating the consequences of reproductive skew for primate behavior. Moreover, a

678

primate perspective on reproductive skew should help to ground models of skew more

679

firmly, specifically in the context of multiple competitors and queuing within groups.

680 681

SUMMARY

682

In this chapter, we considered the causes and consequences of skew in male

683

primates. Although our understanding of the causes of skew is still in its infancy,

684

empirical studies thus far support the compromise framework (tug-of-war model) rather

685

than the concession model.

686

the priority of access (POA) model makes predictions that are very similar to the

687

compromise framework, but that skew models expand significantly on the POA model

688

by including additional factors that relate to patterns of reproduction within groups. Our

689

phylogenetic comparative analyses on mating skew in male primates also provided

690

supporting data for the tug-of-war model because mating skew decreased as the number

691

of males increased, suggesting that monopolization of females becomes more difficult

692

when there are more rivals. However, there have been no studies that represent strong

693

tests of skew models, possibly because of difficulties in estimating parameters that are

694

necessary for quantitative analyses. Future research could help to clarify the causes of

Our assessment of the different models also suggests that

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

29

695

skew, including development of mathematical models that are more suitable to primate

696

societies, empirical studies based on paternity tests, and comparative approaches to

697

investigate interspecific patterns of skew in other biological systems.

698

Previous studies commonly investigated the causes of skew, but fewer have

699

considered the consequences of skew on other physiological and social parameters. We

700

discussed two examples of how the magnitude of reproductive skew affects other

701

biological traits of interest to behavioral ecologists, focusing on within-group

702

relatedness and sexual transmitted diseases. Of these, it appears that effects on

703

within-group relatedness could have the largest effects on patterns of primate sociality.

704

The introduction of reproductive skew models into primate research is likely to provide

705

new insights to primate social and reproductive behaviour in the future, while a primate

706

perspective is likely to stimulate new skew models.

707 708

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

709

We thank Reinmar Hager and Clara Jones for their invitation to contribute this

710

chapter, and we thank Mike Cant, Sarah Hodge, Kavita Isvaran, Jo Setchell and two

711

anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and discussion. This study was supported

712

by JSPS Research Fellowships, RIKEN Special Post-Doctoral Researchers Program,

713

financed by JSPS core-to-core program HOPE (to NK) and the Max Planck Society (to

714

CN).

715 716

REFERENCES

717

Abernethy, K.A., White, L.J.T. & Wickings, E.J (2002). Hordes of mandrills

718

(Mandrillus sphinx): extreme group size and seasonal male presence. Journal

719

of Zoology, London, 258, 131-137.

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

720 721

Alberts, S.C. (1999).

30

Paternal kin discrimination in wild baboons. Proceedings of the

Royal Society of London, Series B, 266, 1501-1506.

722

Alberts, S. C., Buchan, J. C. & Altmann, J. (2006). Sexual selection in wild baboons:

723

from mating opportunities to paternity success. Animal Behaviour,

724

72:1177–1196

725

Alberts, S.C., Altmann, J. & Wilson, M. L. (1996). Mate guarding constrains foraging

726

activity of male baboons. Animal Behaviour, 51:1269–1277

727

Alberts, S.C., Watts, H.E. & Altmann, J. (2003). Queuing and queue-jumping:

728

long-term patterns of reproductive skew in male savannah baboons: Papio

729

cynocephalus. Animal Behaviour, 65, 821–840.

730

Altmann, S.A. (1962). A field study of the sociobiology of the rhesus monkey, Macaca

731

mulatta. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 102, 338–435

732

Altmann, J., Alberts, S.C., Haines, S.A., Dubach, J., Muruthi, P., Coote, T., Geffen, E.,

733

Cheesman, D.J., Mututua, R.S., Saiyalel, S.N., Wayne, R.K., Lacy, R.C. &

734

Bruford, M.W. (1996). Behavior predicts genetic structure in a wild primate

735

group. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States

736

of America, 93, 5797-5801.

737 738 739 740

Anderson, R.M. & May, R.M. (1991). Infectious diseases of humans: dynamics and control. Oxford University Press. Bercovitch, F.B. (1983). Time budgets and consortships in olive baboons (Papio anubis). Folia Primatologica 41, 180–190.

741

Bininda-Emonds, O.R.P. (ed.) 2004. Phylogenetic Supertrees: Combining Information

742

to Reveal the Tree of Life. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, The

743

Netherlands.

744

Blomberg, S. P., and T. Garland Jr. 2002. Tempo and mode in evolution: phylogenetic

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

31

745

inertia, adaptation and comparative methods. Journal of Evolutionary Biology

746

15, 899-910.

747

Boesch, C., Kohou, G., Nene, H. & Vigilant, L. (2006). Male competition and paternity

748

in wild chimpanzees of the Tai forest. American Journal Physical

749

Anthropology, 130, 103-115.

750 751

Boomsma, J. J. & Sundström, L. (1998). Patterns of paternity skew in Formica ants. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 42, 85-92.

752

Boots, M. & Knell, R.J. (2002). The evolution of risky behaviour in the presence of a

753

sexually transmitted disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series

754

B, 269, 585-589.

755

Borries, C.K.L., Epplen, C., Epplen J.T. & Winkler, P. (1999a). Males as infant

756

protectors in Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus) living in multimale groups-

757

defense pattern, paternity and sexual behavior. Behavioral Ecology and

758

Sociobiology, 46, 350-356.

759

Borries, C., Launhardt, K., Epplen, C., Epplen, J.T. & Winkler, P. (1999b). DNA

760

analyses support the hypothesis that infanticide is adaptive in langur monkeys.

761

Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 266, 901-904.

762 763

Bradley, B.J., Doran-Sheehy, D.M., Lukas, D., Boesch, C. & Vigilant, L. (2004). Dispersed male networks in western gorillas. Current Biology, 14, 510-513.

764

Bradley, B.J., Robbins, M.M., Williamson, E.A., Steklis, H.D., Steklis, N.G., Eckhardt,

765

N., Boesch, C. & Vigilant, L. (2005). Mountain gorilla tug-of-war: silverbacks

766

have limited control over reproduction in multimale groups. Proceedings of

767

the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102,

768

9418-9423

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

769 770 771 772 773 774 775 776 777 778 779 780 781

Buchan, J.C., Alberts, S.C., Silk, J.B. & Altmann, J. (2003). True paternal care in a multi-male primate society. Nature, 425, 179-181 Bulger, J.B. (1993). Dominance rank and access to estrous females in male savanna baboons. Behaviour, 127, 67–103 Canfield, P.J., Love, D.N., Mearns, G. & Farram, E. (1991). Chlamydial infection in a colony of captive koalas. Australian Veterinary Journal, 68, 167-169 Cant, M.A. (1998). A model for the evolution of reproductive skew without reproductive suppression. Animal Behaviour, 55, 163–169 Cant, M.A. & English, S. (2006) Stable group size in cooperative breeders: the role of inheritance and reproductive skew. Behavioral Ecology, 17, 560-568 Cant, M.A. & Reeve, H.K. (2002). Female control of the distribution of paternity in cooperative breeders. American Naturalist 160, 602–611 Cant, M. A. & Shen, S. F. (2006). Endogenous timing in competitive interactions

782

among relatives. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 273,

783

171-178

784 785 786

32

Chapais, B. & Berman, C.M. (2003). Kinship and behavior in primates. Oxford University Press, Oxford Charpentier, M., Peignot, P., Hossaert-McKey, M., Gimenez, O., Setchell, J.M. &

787

Wickings, E.J. (2005a). Constraints on control: factors influencing

788

reproductive success in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Behavioral

789

Ecology, 16, 614-623.

790

Charpentier, M., Setchell, J.M., Prugnolle, F., Knapp, L., Wickings, E.J., Peignot, P. &

791

Hossaert-McKey, M. (2005b). Genetic diversity and reproductive success and

792

in mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Proceedings of the National Academy of

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

793 794 795

Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 16723-16728. Clutton-Brock, T.H. (1998). Reproductive skew, concessions and limited control. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 13, 288–292

796

Constable, J., Ashley, M., Goodall, J. & Pusey, A. (2001). Noninvasive paternity

797

assignment in Gombe chimpanzees. Molecular Ecology 10, 1279–1300

798 799 800

33

Cowlishaw, G. & Dunbar, R.I.M. (1991). Dominance rank and mating success in male primates. Animal Behaviour, 41, 1045–1056 Crockett, C.M. & Janson, C.H. (2000). Infanticide in red howlers: female group size,

801

male membership, and a possible link to folivory. In: Infanticide by males and

802

its implications, eds. C.P. van Schaik, & C.H. Janson, pp. 75-98. Cambridge

803

University Press, Cambridge.

804

Curie-Cohen, M., Yoshihara, D., Luttrell, L., Benforado, K., MacCluer, J. W. & Stone,

805

W. H. (1983). The effects of dominance on mating behavior and paternity in a

806

captive troop of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of

807

Primatology, 5, 127-138

808 809

Duffy, J.E., Morrison, C.L. & Rios, R. (2000). Multiple origins of eusociality among sponge-dwelling shrimps (Synalpheus). Evolution, 54, 503-516

810

Dunbar, R.I.M. (1987). Habitat quality, population dynamics, and group composition in

811

colobus monkeys (Colobus guereza). International Journal of Primatology, 8,

812

299-329.

813

Dunbar, R.I.M. (1988). Primate social systems. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY.

814

Ellis, L. (1995). Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: a

815 816 817

cross-species comparison. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 257--333. Faulkes, C.G., Bennett, N.C., Bruford, M.W., O’Brien, P.O. Aguilar, G.H. & Jarvis, J.U.M. (1997). Ecological constraints drive social evolution in the African

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

818

mole-rats. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 264,

819

1619-1627.

820 821 822 823

34

Felsenstein, J. (1985). Phylogenies and the comparative method. American Naturalist, 125, 1-15. Graves, B.M. & Duvall, D. (1995). Effects of sexually-transmitted diseases on heritable variation in sexually selected systems. Animal Behaviour, 50, 1129-1131.

824

Hager, R. (2003). Models of reproductive skew applied to primates. In: Sexual selection

825

and reproductive competition in primates: new perspectives and directions, ed.

826

C.B. Jones. pp 65–101. American Society of Primatologists, Norman,

827

Oklahoma

828 829 830 831

Hamilton, W.D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I.. and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1-16, 17-52. Harvey, P.H. & Pagel, M.D. (1991). The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

832

Haydock, J. & Koenig, W.D. (2002). Reproductive skew in the polygynandrous acorn

833

woodpecker. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United

834

States of America, 99, 7178–7183

835

Heinze, J. (1995). Reproductive skew and genetic relatedness in Leptothorax ants.

836

Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 261, 375-379

837

Hrdy, S.B. & Whitten, P.L. (1987). Patterning of sexual activity. In Primate societies,

838

eds. B.B. Smuts, D.L. Cheney, R.W. Seyfarth, R.W. Wrangham & T.T.

839

Struhsaker, pp 370-384, University of Chicago Press.

840

Inoue, M., Mitsunaga, F., Ohsawa, H., Takenaka, A., Sugiyama, Y., Gaspard, S. A. &

841

Takenaka, O. (1991). Male mating behaviour and paternity discrimination by

842

DNA fingerprinting in a Japanese macaque group. Folia Primatologica, 56,

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

843 844

35

202-210 Inoue, M., Mitsunaga, F., Nozaki, M., Ohsawa, H., Takenaka, A., Sugiyama, Y.,

845

Shimizu, K. & Takenaka, O. (1993). Male dominance rank and reproductive

846

success in an enclosed group of Japanese macaques: with special reference to

847

post-conception mating. Primates, 34, 503-511

848 849 850 851 852 853 854 855

Johnstone, R.A. (2000). Models of reproductive skew: a review and synthesis. Ethology, 106, 5–26 Johnstone, R.A., Woodroffe, R., Cant, M.A. & Wright, J. (1999). Reproductive skew in multimember groups. American Naturalist, 153, 315-331. Keller, L. & Reeve, H.K. (1994). Partitioning of reproduction in animal societies. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 9, 98-102 Kokko, H. & Lindström, J. (1997) Measuring mating skew. American Naturalist 149, 794–799

856

Kokko, H. & Johnstone, R.A. (1999). Social queuing in animal societies: a dynamic

857

model of reproductive skew. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London,

858

Series B, 265, 571-578

859 860

Kokko, H., Ranta, E., Ruxton, G. & Lundberg, P. (2002). Sexually transmitted disease and the evolution of mating systems. Evolution, 56, 1091-1100.

861

Kutsukake, N. & Nunn, C.L. (2006). Comparative tests of reproductive skew in male

862

primates: the roles of demographic factors and incomplete control. Behavioral

863

Ecology and Sociobiology, 60, 695-706.

864

Lockhart, A.B., Thrall, P.H. & Antonovics, J. (1996). Sexually transmitted diseases in

865

animals: ecological and evolutionary implications. Biological Reviews of the

866

Cambridge Philosophical Society, 71, 415-471.

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

36

867

Magrath, R.D., Heinsohn, R.G. & Johnstone, R.A. (2004). Reproductive skew. In:

868

Ecology and Evolution of Cooperative Breeding in Birds, eds. W.D. Koenig &

869

J.L. Dickinson, pp 157-176, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

870

McNamara, J.M., Gasson, C.E. & Houston, A.I. (1999). Incorporating rules for

871 872

responding into evolutionary games. Nature 401, 368–371. Mesterton-Gibbons, M., Hardy, I.C.W., Field, J. (2006). The effect of differential

873

survivorship on the stability of reproductive queueing. Journal of Theoretical

874

Biology 242, 699-712.

875

Muniz, L., Perry, S., Manson, J.M., Gilkenson, H., Gros-Louis, J. & Vigilant, L. (2006).

876

Father-daughter inbreeding avoidance in a wild primate population. Current

877

Biology 16, R156-157.

878

Nerrienet, E., Amouretti, X., Muller-Trutwin, M.C., Poaty-Mavoungou, V., Bedjebaga,

879

I., Nguyen, H.T., Dubreuil G., Corbet, S., Wickings, E.J., Barre-Sinoussi, F.,

880

Georges, A.J. & Georges-Courbot, M.C. (1998). Phylogenetic analysis of SIV

881

and STLV type I in mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx): Indications that intracolony

882

transmissions are predominantly the result of male-to-male aggressive contacts.

883

Aids Research and Human Retroviruses, 14, 785-796.

884

Nishida, T., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Hasegawa, T. & Takahata, Y. (1985). Group

885

extinction and female transfer in wild chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains.

886

Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 67, 284-301.

887 888 889 890 891

Nonacs, P. (2000). Measuring and using skew in the study of social behavior and evolution. American Naturalist, 156, 577– 589 Nonacs, P. (2003). Measuring the reliability of skew indices: is there one best index? Animal Behaviour, 65, 615-627. van Noordwijk, M.A. & van Schaik, C.P. (2004). Sexual selection and the careers of

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

37

892

primate males: paternity concentration, dominance acquisition tactics and

893

transfer decisions. In Sexual Selection in Primates: a Comparative Perspective,

894

eds. P.M. Kappeler & C.P. van Schaik, pp 208-229, Cambridge University

895

Press, Cambridge

896

Nunn, C.L. (1999a). The number of males in primate social groups: a comparative test

897

of the socioecological model. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 46, 1–13

898

Nunn, C.L. (1999b). The evolution of exaggerated sexual swellings in primates and the

899 900

graded signal hypothesis. Animal Behaviour, 58, 229-246. Nunn, C.L. & Altizer, S.M. (2004). Sexual selection, behavior and sexually transmitted

901

diseases. In Sexual Selection in Primates: a Comparative Perspective, eds. P.M.

902

Kappeler & C.P. van Schaik, pp. 117-130. Cambridge University Press,

903

Cambridge.

904 905 906 907 908

Nunn, C.L. & Altizer, S.M. (2006). Infectious diseases in primates: behavior, ecology and evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Nunn, C.L. & Barton, R.A. (2001). Comparative methods for studying primate adaptation and allometry. Evolutionary Anthropology, 10, 81-98 Ohsawa, H., Inoue, M. & Takenaka, O. (1993). Mating strategy and reproductive

909

success of male patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas). Primates, 34, 533-544

910

Packer, C. (1979). Male dominance and reproductive activity in Papio anubis. Animal

911

Behaviour, 37, 37–45

912

Paul, A. (1997). Breeding seasonality affects the association between dominance and

913

reproductive success in non-human male primates. Folia Primatologica 68,

914

344-349.

915

Paul, A. & Kuester, J. (1996). Differential reproduction in male and female Barbary

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

916

macaques. In: Fa, J. E. & Lindburg, D. G. (eds) Evolution and ecology of

917

macaque societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 293-317

918

Pope, T. R. (1990). The reproductive consequences of male cooperation in the red

919

howler monkey: paternity exclusion in multi-male and single-male troops

920

using genetic markers. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 27, 439-446

921

Ragsdale, J.E. (1999). Reproductive skew theory extended: The effect of resource

922

inheritance on social organization. Evolutionary Ecological Research, 1,

923

859-874

924 925

38

Reeve, H.K. & Emlen, S.T. (2000). Reproductive skew and group size: an N-person staying incentive model. Behavioral Ecology, 11, 640-647

926

Reeve, H.K., Emlen, S.T. & Keller, L. (1998). Reproductive sharing in animal societies:

927

reproductive incentives or incomplete control by dominant breeders?

928

Behavioral Ecology, 9, 267–278

929

Reeve, H.K. & Ratnieks, F.L.W. (1993). Queen-queen conflicts in polygynous societies:

930

mutual tolerance and reproductive skew. In: Queen Number and Sociality in

931

Insects. ed. L. Keller., P45-85. New York, Oxford University Press.

932

Reeve, H.K. & Shen, S-F. (2006). A missing model in reproductive skew theory: The

933

bordered tug-of-war. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the

934

United States of America, 103, 8430-8434

935

Rendall, D. (2004). ‘Recognizing’ kin: Mechanisms, media, minds, modules and

936

muddles. In: Kinship and Behaviour in Primates. Eds. B. Chapais & C.

937

Berman, Pp. 295-316. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

938 939 940

Ridley, M. (1986). The number of males in a primate troop. Animal Behaviour, 34, 1848-1858 Robbins, M.M., Sicotte, P. & Stewart, K.J. 2001. Mountain gorillas: three decades of

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

941 942

research at Karisoke. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Robbins, A.M. & Robbins, M.M. (2005). Fitness consequences of dispersal decisions

943

for male mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei). Behavioral Ecology

944

and Sociobiology, 58, 295-309.

945

39

de Ruiter, J. R., van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. & Scheffrahn, W. (1994). Social and genetic

946

aspects of paternity in wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis).

947

Behaviour, 129, 203-224

948

Say, L., Pontier, D. & Natoli, E. (2001). Influence of oestrus synchronization on male

949

reproductive success in the domestic cat (Felis catus L.). Proceedings of the

950

Royal Society of London, Series B, 268, 1049-1053

951

van Schaik, C.P., Hodges, K. & Nunn, C.L. (2000). Paternity confusion and the ovarian

952

cycles of female primates. In: Infanticide by males and its implications. Eds. C.P.

953

van Schaik & C.H. Janson, pp. 361-387. Cambridge: Cambridge University

954

Press.

955 956

van Schaik, C.P. & Janson, C.H (2001). Infanticide by males and its implications. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

957

van Schaik, C.P., van Noordwijk, M.A. & Nunn, C.L. (1999). Sex and social evolution

958

in primates. In: Comparative socioecology of primates, ed. P. Lee, pp 204-240,

959

Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

960

Setchell, J.M., Charpentier, M. & Wickings, E.J. (2005). Mate-guarding and paternity in

961

mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx): factors influencing monopolisation of females by

962

the alpha male. Animal Behaviour, 70, 1105-1120.

963

Sherman, P.W., Lacey, E.A., Reeve, H.K. & Keller, L. (1995). The eusociality

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

964 965 966 967 968 969

continuum. Behavioral Ecology, 6, 102-108 Shively, C. & Smith, D. G. (1985). Social status and reproductive success of male Macaca fascicularis. American Journal of Primatology, 9, 129-135 Shuster, S.M. & Wade, M.J. (2003). Mating systems and mating strategies. Princeton University Press, Princeton, N.J. Silk, J. B., Altmann, J. & Alberts, S. C. (2006). Social relationships among adult female

970

baboons (Papio cynocephalus) I. Variation in the strength of social bonds.

971

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

972 973

40

Smith, D. G. (1981). The association between rank and reproductive success of male rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 1, 83-90

974

Smith, K., Alberts, S.C. & Altmann, J. (2003). Wild female baboons bias their social

975

behaviour towards paternal half-sisters. Proceedings of the Royal Society of

976

London, Series B, 270, 503 – 510

977

Soltis, J., Mitsunaga, F., Shimizu, K., Nozaki, M., Yanagihara, Y., Domingo-Roura, X.

978

& Takenaka, O. (1997). Sexual selection in Japanese macaques II: female mate

979

choice and male-male competition. Animal Behaviour, 54, 737-746

980

Soltis, J., Thomsen, R., Matsubayashi, K. & Takenaka, O. (2000). Infanticide by

981

resident males and female counter-strategies in wild Japanese macaques

982

(Macaca fuscata). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 48, 195 – 202

983

Soltis, J., Thomsen, R. & Takenaka, O. (2001). The interaction of male and female

984

reproductive strategies and paternity in wild Japanese macaques, Macaca

985

fuscata. Animal Behaviour, 62, 485–494

986

Soltis, J. (2004). Mating systems. In: Macaque Societies: a model for the study of social

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

41

987

organization. Eds. B, Thierry. M. Singh, & W. Kaumanns, pp: 135-151,

988

Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

989

Srivastava, A. & Dunbar, R.I.M. (1996). The mating system of Hanuman langurs: a

990

problem in optimal foraging. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 39,

991

219-226

992

Takahashi, H. (2004). Do males have a better chance of mating when the number of

993

estrous females is equal to or greater than the males' ordinal rank? Testing the

994

hypothesis in Japanese macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 63,

995

95-102

996

Thrall, P.H., Antonovics, J. & Bever, J.D. (1997). Sexual transmission of disease and

997

host mating systems: within-season reproductive success. American Naturalist,

998

149, 485-506.

999

Thrall, P.H., Antonovics, J. & Dobson, A.P. (2000). Sexually transmitted diseases in

1000

polygynous mating systems:

1001

Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 267, 1555-1563.

prevalence and impact on reproductive success.

1002

Treves, A. (2001). Reproductive consequences of variation in the composition of howler

1003

monkey (Alouatta spp.) groups. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50,

1004

61-71.

1005 1006 1007 1008 1009

Vehrencamp, S.L. (1983a). A model for the evolution of despotic versus egalitarian societies. Animal Behaviour, 31, 667–682 Vehrencamp, S.L. (1983b). Optimal skew in cooperative societies. American Zoologist, 23, 327-355. Watts, D.P. (2000). Causes and consequences of variation in male mountain gorilla life.

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

42

1010

histories and group membership. In: Primate males: causes and consequences

1011

of variation in group composition. Ed. P.M. Kappeler, pp. 169-180. Cambridge

1012

University Press, Cambridge.

1013

Widdig, A., Bercovitch, F.B., Streich, W.J., Sauermann, U., Nuernberg, P. & Krawczak,

1014

M. (2004). A longitudinal analysis of reproductive skew in male macaques.

1015

Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 271, 819–826

1016

Widdig, A., Nürnberg, P., Krawczak, M., Streich, W.J. & Bercovitch, F.B. (2001).

1017

Paternal relatedness and age proximity regulate social relationships among

1018

adult female rhesus macaques. Proceedings of the National Academy of

1019

Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 13769-13773

1020

Williams, D.A. (2004). Female control of reproductive skew in cooperatively breeding

1021

brown jays (Cyanocorax morio). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 55,

1022

370-380

1023 1024

Wrangham, R.W. (1999). The evolution of coalitionary killing. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 42, 1–30.

43

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

1 2

Table 1. Predicted relationships between the reproductive skew and the number of males and females, oestrous overlap, and relatedness among males from three models Effects on reproductive skew The

The ‘extended’

priority-of-access

priority-of-access

model

model

The tug-of-war model

model

Number of males in group

No prediction

-

-

No prediction

Number of females in group

No prediction

No prediction

-

No prediction

-

-

-

No prediction

No prediction

No prediction

No prediction or -

+

Oestrous overlap Relatedness among males 3

Models

The concession

+: positive relationship between variable and degree of skew; -: negative relationship with skew.

44

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

4

Table 2. Summary of empirical studies investigating reproductive skew in male primates Model

The

The

of

priority of

tug-of-

conces

Other

oestrous

access

access

war

sion

important

male

female

among males

overlap

model

model

model

model

factors

ns

Yes

No

Hetero-

Cayo Sandiago,

ns

Macaca mulatta

Puerto Rico

(but two most

(free-ranging

successful

provisioned)

males were related in two of five years)

colony, Gabon;

‘extended’

Relatedness

Rhesus macaque a

Mandrillus sphinx

priority

#

Study site, group

CIRMF mandrill

The

# Species

Mandrill b, c

The

zygosity

45

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

(semi-free-ranging provisioned) Setchell et al. 2005

-

Charpentier et al.

+

-

-b

Yes

Yes

Yes

No

-

Yes

Partially

Yes

No

Yes f

2005a mountain gorilla c

Karisoke, Virunga,

Gorilla gorilla

Volcanoes National

ns

ns

+ in one (of

not

four) group

investigated,

Park, Rwanda

but overlap

(wild)

is unlikely

Chimpanzee d

Taï National Park in -

-

Pan troglodytes

Cote d’Ivoire (wild)

Comparative

-

ns

ns

analyses e (31 species) 5

+: positive relationship; -: negative relationship; ns: uncorrelated

ns

Yes

avoidance Yes

No

Yes

Yes

No

Partially

Yes

No

Yes f

Incest

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

6

a: Widdig et al. 2004; b: controlled for the effect of oestrous overlap by calculating the deviance of observed data from expected value

7

from the priority-of-access model; c: Bradley et al. 2005; d: Boesch et al. 2006; e: Kutsukake and Nunn, 2006; f: “partial”

8

because the effect of oestrous overlap was not confirmed.

46

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

47

1

Figure legends

2

Fig. 1. Scheme of the causes and consequences of reproductive skew discussed in this

3

chapter. Black arrows indicate the effects predicted from the compromise framework

4

(i.e., tug-of-war model), and the dashed arrows indicate the effects predicted from the

5

transactional framework (i.e., concession model).

6 7

Fig. 2. Phylogenetic comparative analyses on the relationship between male number in

8

a group and the maximum mating proportion (the proportion of mating by the most

9

successful male). For further details on these analyses, see Kutsukake and Nunn (2006).

10 11

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

12

13

(Figure 1)

48

Kutsukake and Nunn: reproductive skew in male primates

14

15 16

(Figure 2)

49

Skew chapter

Tel: +81-48-462-1111-6823, fax: +81-48-467-7503. E-mail: ..... expectation that related subordinate males can receive their “staying incentive” in the ... provided by research from a semi-free-ranging captive colony (CIRMF Mandrill. Colony ...

226KB Sizes 3 Downloads 191 Views

Recommend Documents

The left-liberal skew of Western media
Academics left-wing too and not interested in media bias because it benefits themselves. ○ But some can be found in more obscure sources: ○ Dissertations.

Comparative tests of reproductive skew in male primates
Aug 11, 2006 - available in the online version of this article at http://dx.doi. .... costly to males in terms of time, energy, and opportunity costs (Packer 1979; Bercovitch ...... Paternity analysis of alternative male reproductive routes among the

On the growth problem for skew and symmetric ...
Abstract. C. Koukouvinos, M. Mitrouli and Jennifer Seberry, in “Growth in Gaussian elimi- nation for weighing matrices, W(n, n − 1)”, Linear Algebra and its Appl., 306 (2000),. 189-202, conjectured that the growth factor for Gaussian eliminatio

Comparative tests of reproductive skew in male primates
Aug 11, 2006 - Graduate School of Sciences, University of Tokyo,. Tokyo ... In strict application of the priority of ...... Princeton University Press, Princeton.

Chapter Tour Chapter
Pictures with captions/ Info graphics: Charts and/or maps (page with title):. Biography (People, some info):. Chapter Objectives: Primary Source Documents (Title ...

Chapter 1.2 Chapter 1.4.1
Disk Operating System (DOS). What is DOS, and why learn about it? Microsoft developed the Disk Operating System (DOS) in 1981. DOS, which is sometimes called MS-DOS, was designed for the IBM PC. Windows 98 and Windows. 2000 both support DOS commands

chapter p chapter 1
Write the product in standard form. 5) (3 + 5i)(2 + 9i). 5). Find the product of the complex number and its conjugate. 6) -1 - 5i. 6). CHAPTER 1. Find the domain of ...

CHAPTER ONE
MAPS. 1. The VOC territories in Austronesian-speaking Asia, ca. the 1660s. 2. Indigenous ethno-linguistic groups of Taiwan. 3. Geographic distribution of ...

Chapter 5
not in the domain. The only critical point is x = 0. As x moves away from 0 on either side, the values of y decrease. The function has a local maximum value at (0, ...... (b) Since. ,. dV. dV dr dt dr dt. = we have. 2 . dV dr rh dt dt π. = (c). 2. 2

Chapter 15
373 cancelled each other and there is zero displacement throughout. To put the principle of superposition mathematically, let y1 (x,t) and y2 (x,t) be the displacements due to two wave disturbances in the medium. If the waves arrive in a region simul

Chapter 9
9.1 Introduction. In mathematics, the word, “sequence” is used in much the same way as it is in ordinary English. When we say that a collection of objects is listed ...

Chapter 09
In the late 1700s and early 1800s, he kept notes about his travels .... In 1762, he quit the company. In 1670, the Hudson's Bay Company held trading rights and.

Chapter 15
The most familiar type of waves such as waves on a string, water waves, sound waves, seismic waves, etc. is the so-called mechanical waves. These waves require a medium for propagation, they cannot propagate through vacuum. They involve oscillations

Physics 235 Chapter 3 - 1 - Chapter 3 Oscillations In this Chapter ...
In this Chapter different types of oscillations will be discussed. A particle carrying out oscillatory motion, oscillates around a stable equilibrium position (note: if ...

Chapter 1 – Getting Started Chapter 2 - PSM ... - GCAP CoolCast
What is Garden City Ammonia Program? What is Refrigeration? Why Refrigeration? Why Take an Operator I Course? Is there a Career in the Industrial ...

ACF Central Florida Chapter Named Southeast Region Chapter of the ...
Mar 31, 2016 - Page 1 ... or on Facebook at www. ... Plugrá® European-Style Butter; Vitamix; Ecolab; Allen Brothers; Wisconsin Milk Marketing Board; Atlantic ...

ACF Central Florida Chapter Named Southeast Region Chapter of the ...
Mar 31, 2016 - ... Vitamix; Ecolab; Allen Brothers; Wisconsin Milk Marketing Board; Atlantic Veal & Lamb;. American Technical Publishers; Par-Way Tryson Company; The ... for chefs in the United States, with the Certified Executive Chef®, ... ACF on

Chapter 1 – Getting Started Chapter 2 - PSM ... - GCAP CoolCast
How much Must I Know about Process Safety Management to be an Operator? Are there Any Organizations that Can Help Me in ... “To the Extent they can affect the process” Mean? How do I Properly Document this Training? ... are some Chemical Characte

Chapter 9_86-117p.pdf
These books have Spirit for theme. I shall never ... He said: 'I will make each of them threefold.' He and life .... "My son Bees create honey by gathering the sweet.

Chapter 3
The 4 step numbers in the example below, are also labels ... 3 • 2 = 6 , is just the point 3 on a number line, being scaled by 2 (made twice as far from the origin).

Chapter
order to communicate alarms from patient monitoring and therapeutic ... After implementation of the central application (AM), as specified in the ACM profile,.

Chapter
SPSS (version 12.0, SPSS Inc.) was used for all analysis. .... ence of prehospital ECG predictive of a reduced door-to-balloon time (mean ± SE) by 38.8 ... Lastly, a multivariate logistic regression model was constructed to determine the sig-.