MEMORY, 2006, 14 (5), 637 647

Phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories for social and non-social events in social phobia Arnaud D’Argembeau University of Lie`ge, Belgium Martial Van der Linden University of Geneva, Switzerland, and University of Lie`ge, Belgium Mathieu d’Acremont University of Geneva, Switzerland Isabelle Mayers University of Lie`ge, Belgium Previous studies failed to show clear differences between people with social phobia and non-anxious individuals regarding the specificity and affective intensity of their autobiographical memories for social events. However, these studies did not assess the subjective experience associated with remembering. In this study, people with social phobia and non-anxious control participants recalled social and non-social events, and rated the phenomenal characteristics of their memories. The memories of people with social phobia for social events contained fewer sensorial details but more self-referential information than controls’ memories. In addition, people with social phobia remembered social situations from an observer perspective, viewing themselves as if from outside, to a greater extent than controls. By contrast, the two groups did not differ concerning their memories for non-social events. These findings are discussed in relation to cognitive models of social phobia.

Cognitive models of social phobia assume that differences in how individuals process social information play a causal role in the development or maintenance of the disorder (Clark & Wells, 1995; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997). According to Clark and Wells (1995; see also Clark & McManus, 2002), people with social phobia tend to appraise social situations as being excessively dangerous. Consequently, when entering a social

situation, they shift attention away from the situation and become highly self-focused, they use a variety of cognitive and behavioural strategies to try to prevent the catastrophes they fear from occurring, and they process external social cues in a biased manner (i.e., threatening information is given priority). The model further hypothesises that people with social phobia selectively retrieve negative information about

Address correspondence to: Arnaud D’Argembeau, Cognitive Psychopathology Unit, University of Lie`ge, Boulevard du Rectorat B33, 4000 Lie`ge, Belgium. E-mail: [email protected] This work was supported by the Government of the French Community of Belgium (Direction de la Recherche Scientifique  Actions de Recherche Concerte´es, Convention 99/04-246). Arnaud D’Argembeau is a Postdoctoral Researcher at the Belgian National Fund for Scientific Research (FNRS). We would like to thank Be´ne´dicte Lejeune for her help in participant recruitment and testing.

# 2006 Psychology Press Ltd http://www.psypress.com/memory

DOI:10.1080/09658210600747183

638

D’ARGEMBEAU ET AL.

themselves and their social performance, thus maintaining negative images of themselves and negative expectations regarding future social situations. In accordance with this model, substantial evidence indicates that people with social phobia show attentional biases towards perceived threats (e.g., Becker, Rinck, Margraf, & Roth, 2001; Hope, Rapee, Heimberg, & Dombeck, 1990; Musa, Le´pine, Clark, Mansell, & Ehlers, 2003) and interpret ambiguous stimuli as threatening (e.g., Amir, Foa, & Coles, 1998; Stopa & Clark, 2000). By contrast, the evidence for an explicit memory bias in social phobia is mixed. Although some studies have found that non-clinical individuals with high social anxiety recalled more negative self-referent trait words (Breck & Smith, 1983; O’Banion & Arkowitz, 1977) or fewer positive self-referent trait words (Mansell & Clark, 1999) than individuals with low social anxiety, the majority of studies that assessed memory for verbal stimuli have failed to find a memory bias either in people with social phobia (Becker, Roth, Andrich, & Margraf, 1999; Brendle & Wenzel, 2004; Cloitre, Cancienne, Heimberg, Holt, & Liebowitz, 1995; Rapee, McCallum, Melville, Ravenscroft, & Rodney, 1994) or in non-clinical individuals with high social anxiety (Sanz, 1996). On the other hand, other studies report that people with social phobia or non-clinical individuals with high social anxiety show a better memory for negative facial expressions (Foa, Gilboa-Schechtman, Amir, & Freshman, 2000; but see Pe´rez-Lopez & Woody, 2001) or a poorer memory for positive expressions (D’Argembeau, Van der Linden, Etienne, & Comblain, 2003). Two studies also report that people with social phobia recognise more faces that have been rated as ‘‘critical’’ rather than ‘‘accepting’’ (Coles & Heimberg, 2005; Lundh & ¨ st, 1996), but signal detection analyses suggest O that this finding results from differences in response bias (C ) rather than differences in memory accuracy (Coles & Heimberg, 2005). Surprisingly, few studies have examined whether social phobia is associated with memory biases for social events experienced in daily life (i.e., autobiographical memory). Rapee et al. (1994, Study 4) asked participants with social phobia and non-anxious participants to describe the first memory that came to mind in response to cue words related to social situations and to cue words unrelated to social situations. People with social phobia did not recall more negative social

events than non-anxious participants, and the two groups did not differ in terms of the degree of anxiety associated with the memories. More recently, Wenzel, Jackson, and Holt (2002) investigated whether social phobia affected the specificity of memories for social events. People with social phobia and non-anxious participants were asked to recall memories in response to cue words that were related or unrelated to social situations, and the memories were then coded by independent raters for their specific or general nature (to be specific, a memory had to involve an event located at a particular time and place and lasting no longer than one day), as well as for their positive, negative, or neutral character. Although people with social phobia did not retrieve a higher percentage of specific memories cued by social words than control participants, they recalled more memories characterized by negative affect. However, in a subsequent study, Wenzel, Werner, Cochran, and Holt (2004) found that non-anxious participants retrieved fewer general memories and more memories characterised by negative affect when cued with social threat words than when cued with positive words, while there were no differences for participants with social phobia. On the other hand, Field, Psychol, and Morgan (2004) reported that, compared to non-anxious controls, non-clinical individuals with high social anxiety recalled memories that were rated as significantly more negative and shameful. Finally, a questionnaire study reported by Rachman, Gru¨ter-Andrew, and Shafran (2000) indicated that past negative social events were remembered more often by non-clinical individuals with high social anxiety than by individuals with low social anxiety, and that these memories had a more intrusive quality. Overall, as is the case with memory biases for verbal stimuli, the evidence for autobiographical memory biases in social phobia is mixed. However, the subjective aspects that are associated with remembering an event such as the phenomenal characteristics of memories (e.g., the amount of sensorial and contextual details experienced by the individual while remembering) have not been investigated in these earlier studies. Yet the subjective experience associated with remembering an event is a fundamental aspect of autobiographical/episodic memory (Conway, 2001; Rubin, Schrauf, & Greenberg, 2003; Wheeler, Stuss, & Tulving, 1997), which influences people’s decisions about the origin of their memories (e.g., determining whether an event really happened or

AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY IN SOCIAL PHOBIA

was merely imagined; Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993) and grounds self-knowledge in remembered reality (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Conway, Singer, & Tagini, 2004). By restricting memory measures to quantitative aspects (e.g., the number of words or memories recalled and the time taken to retrieve these memories), one may miss differences in the qualitative aspects of the memories of people with social phobia and non-anxious individuals. A notable exception to the lack of studies investigating the phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories in social phobia pertains to the visual perspective adopted in the memories (Nigro & Neisser, 1983). Indeed, several studies have indicated that, when remembering social situations, people with social phobia are more likely than non-anxious individuals to take an observer perspective, viewing themselves as if from an external point of view, whereas this is not the case for non-social situations (Coles, Turk, Heimberg, & Fresco, 2001; Hackmann, Surawy, & Clark, 1998; Wells, Clark, & Ahmad, 1998; Wells & Papageorgiou, 1999). Some evidence further indicates that remembering visual images of the self from an observer perspective while being in a social situation increases anxiety and worsens performance during this situation (Hirsch, Clark, Mathews, & Williams, 2003; Hirsch, Meynen, & Clark, 2004; Spurr & Stopa, 2003). Together, these findings are encouraging, as they suggest that differences in the subjective experience of remembering may play an important role in maintaining social phobia. Given the paucity of experimental data concerning the influence of social phobia on the subjective experience associated with remembering social events, the purpose of the present study was to investigate potential differences between people with social phobia and non-anxious individuals regarding the phenomenal characteristics of their memories for social events. People with social phobia and non-anxious control participants were asked to remember two social events (one positive, one negative) and two non-social events (one positive, one negative) and to rate their memories according to various dimensions (amount of sensorial and contextual details, amount of self-referential information, amount of information concerning others, visual perspective while remembering). Following Clark and Wells’s (1995) suggestion that social phobia is associated with self-focused attention during social situations, we predicted that the memories

639

of people with social phobia for social events (and especially negative ones) would contain more self-referential information but less information concerning others, as well as fewer sensorial and contextual details than non-anxious participants’ memories. In addition, in light of previous studies of this issue (Coles et al., 2001; Hackmann et al., 1998; Wells et al., 1998; Wells & Papageorgiou, 1999), we predicted that people with social phobia would be more likely to adopt an observer perspective than non-anxious participants when remembering social situations (and especially negative ones), but not when remembering nonsocial situations.

METHOD Participants The participants were 17 individuals (5 women, 12 men) with generalised social phobia (GSP) who sought treatment for their social fears and met the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) diagnosis of GSP as determined by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI; Lecrubier, Weiller, Bonora, Amorin, & Le´pine, 1994; Sheehan et al., 1998). The nonanxious group consisted of 17 participants (5 women, 12 men) with no known history of psychological disorders. They were assessed with the MINI in order to confirm that they did not meet DSM-IV criteria for any Axis I disorder. The two groups of participants were matched as closely as possible for age, gender, and educational level. In addition, all participants completed French translations of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (FNE; Watson & Friend, 1969) and the Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996). As can be seen in Table 1, participants with GSP scored higher than non-anxious participants on both measures.

Materials and procedure All participants were tested individually. An initial introduction explained that they would be asked to remember some events that they had personally experienced in the past, and to answer some questions about their memories of these events. Detailed written instructions explained that the events they were to recall had to be precise and specific (i.e., they had to have taken

640

D’ARGEMBEAU ET AL. TABLE 1 Characteristics of people with social phobia and non-anxious participants People with social phobia Age Education FNE BDI-II

35.1 12.8 24.3 18.8

(10.2) (2.9) (5.8) (8.8)

Non-anxious

t (32)

p

35 (10.3) 13 (2.6) 9.7 (5.9) 5.4 (3.4)

0.02 0.19 7.26 5.83

.99 .85 .001 .001

FNE/Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale; BDI-II/Beck Depression Inventory II. Standard deviations are shown in parentheses.

place in a specific location at a specific time and they had to have lasted a few minutes or hours but not more than a day); some examples were provided to illustrate what would or would not be considered as a specific event. The instructions further explained that the participants would be asked to try to remember the events in as much detail as possible (i.e., remembering the setting and course of the events, the persons and objects that were present, and so forth) in order to mentally ‘‘re-experience’’ them. Each participant was then asked to recall two social events (one positive, one negative) and two non-social events (one positive, one negative) that had happened in the recent past (i.e., within the last year, but at least 1 month ago). Non-social events were defined as situations in which participants were alone, whereas social events were defined as situations involving several other people. Positive and negative events were defined as events about which the participants had felt positive emotions (e.g., joy, comfort, or satisfaction) and negative emotions (e.g., fear, discomfort, anxiety, or embarrassment), respectively. The order of presentation of positive and negative social and non-social events was counterbalanced across participants. For each event recalled, participants were first asked to write down a brief description of the event. However, they were free to skip this question; this concession was made in order to prevent a change of memory if a participant considered that it would be embarrassing to report it. Participants then rated their memory on several 7-point rating scales, which were adapted from the Memory Characteristics Questionnaire (Johnson, Foley, Suengas, & Raye, 1988) or created specifically for this study. Memories were rated for visual details (1/none, 7 /a lot), sounds (1 /none, 7/a lot), smell/taste (1/none, 7/a lot), clarity of location (1/not at all clear, 7/very clear), clarity of the spatial arrangement of objects (1 /vague, 7 /clear and distinct),

clarity of the spatial arrangement of people (1 / vague, 7 /clear and distinct), clarity of the time of day (1/not at all clear, 7/very clear), memory of one’s own behaviour during the event (1 / none, 7/very detailed), memory of the behaviour of other people (1 /none, 7/very detailed), memory of what one said during the event (1/none, 7 /very detailed), memory of what other people said (1 /none, 7 /very detailed), and memory of what one thought during the event (1 /none, 7/very detailed). For nonsocial events, the items regarding the spatial arrangement of people, memory of what one said, memory of other people’s behaviour, and memory of what other people said were not included because these events were not supposed to involve other persons. Participants were also asked to report the visual perspective they took in their memory with a 7-point scale, depending on whether they ‘‘saw’’ themselves in their memory (observer perspective) or saw the scene from their own perspective (field perspective; /3 / entirely looking out through my eyes, 3 /entirely observing myself from an outside point of view). A detailed paragraph instructed them on the distinction between the observer and field perspectives (see Nigro & Neisser, 1983). Finally, participants dated the event (in months) and rated the level of anxiety they felt when the event was experienced (1 /no anxiety, 7 /very high anxiety). After all these ratings were completed, participants proceeded to retrieve and assess the next event.

RESULTS Before conducting the analyses, the ratings for visual details, sounds, and smell/taste were averaged into a sensorial details measure (see, e.g., Suengas & Johnson, 1988). Similarly, items regarding clarity of location, clarity of spatial arrangement of objects, and clarity of the time

AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY IN SOCIAL PHOBIA

of day were averaged into a contextual details measure. Items regarding memory for one’s own behaviour, memory for what one said, and memory for what one thought were averaged into a memory for self-referential information measure (for non-social events, this measure only included memory for one’s own behaviour and memory for what one thought). Finally, for social events, items regarding memory of spatial arrangement of people, memory for their behaviours, and memory for what they said were averaged into a memory for other-referential information measure. The mean ratings and standard deviations are presented in Table 2 as a function of group (social phobic, non-anxious), event type (social, nonsocial), and event valence (positive, negative). These ratings were analysed using 2 (group)/2 (event type) /2 (event valence) analyses of variance (ANOVAs), except for memory for other-referential information, which was analysed with a 2 (group) /2 (event valence) ANOVA. An alpha level of .05 was used for these analyses. In addition, effect sizes are reported for the principal outcomes. Specifically, Pearson’s point-biserial correlation (r) was used to evaluate the magnitude of the effect of social phobia on the ratings for phenomenal characteristics (see, e.g., Rosnow, Rosenthal, & Rubin, 2000). The nonanxious group was set at 0 and the social phobic group at 1; thus, a positive correlation indicated a higher rating for social phobia. Estimated correlation coefficients are reported within their 95% confidence interval (CI). For sensorial details, the main effects of group and event type just failed to reach statistical

641

significance, F (1, 32) /3.46, MSE /3.29, p/.07, and F (1, 32) /3.81, MSE /1.81, p/.06, respectively, but the interaction between these two factors was significant, F (1, 32) /5.26, MSE / 1.81, p /.03 (see Figure 1). Planned comparisons revealed that people with social phobia rated memories for social events as containing fewer sensorial details than non-anxious participants, F (1, 32) /14.59, MSE /1.43, p B/.001, r / /.56 (95% CI: /.76 to /.27), whereas this was not the case with memories of non-social events, F (1, 32) /0.01, MSE /3.67, r/ /.02 (95% CI: /.36 to .32). In addition, the main effect of event valence was significant, with memories for positive events containing more sensorial details than memories for negative events, F (1, 32) / 5.52, MSE /1.31, p/.03; this effect did not interact with group, F (1, 32) /0.42, MSE /1.31. The three-way interaction was not significant, F (1, 32) /0.80, MSE /0.59. For contextual details, the ANOVA did not reveal any significant main effect or interaction (all F sB/1.70, ps /.20). Memory for self-referential information was better for negative than for positive events, F(1, 32) /7.97, MSE /1.20, p / .008. In addition, there was a group-by-event-type interaction, F (1, 32)/4.08, MSE /0.72, p/.05. Planned comparisons showed that people with social phobia remembered more self-referential information than non-anxious participants for social events, F (1, 32) /4.27, MSE /1.90, p/ .047, r/.34 (95% CI: .01 to .61), but not for nonsocial events, F (1, 32) /0.09, MSE /2.11, r/.05 (95% CI: /.29 to .38). The three-way interaction was not significant, F(1, 32) /0.39, MSE /1.20. With regard to memory for other-referential

TABLE 2 Mean ratings for phenomenal characteristics in people with social phobia and non-anxious participants as a function of event type and event valence People with social phobia Social events

Sensorial details Contextual details Self-referential information Other-referential information Perspective Anxiety Date

Non-anxious

Non-social events

Social events

Positive

Negative

3.94 (0.27) 5.65 (0.20) 5.35 (0.36)

3.73 (0.23) 5.61 (0.35) 6.15 (0.30)

4.24 (0.33)

4.76 (0.34)

0.64 (0.50) 2.71 (0.40) 2.94 (0.78)

1.47 (0.45) /0.35 (0.54) /1.47 (0.43) /0.05 (0.50) /0.53 (0.45) 6.35 (0.26) 2.65 (0.39) 5.76 (0.31) 1.88 (0.40) 5.35 (0.26) 4.64 (0.92) 2.82 (0.78) 2.88 (0.68) 6.12 (0.78) 5.59 (0.92)

Non-social events

Positive

Negative

Positive

Negative

4.14 (0.40) 6.06 (0.29) 5.53 (0.30)

3.69 (0.35) 6.20 (0.21) 5.88 (0.26)

5.29 (0.27) 6.10 (0.20) 4.82 (0.36)

4.59 (0.23) 5.84 (0.35) 5.29 (0.30)

4.20 (0.40) 5.78 (0.29) 5.35 (0.30)

4.55 (0.33)

5.33 (0.34)





0.47 (0.54) 1.82 (0.39) 4.88 (0.78)

/0.41 (0.43) 5.82 (0.31) 3.94 (0.68)

Standard deviations are shown in parentheses.





Positive

Negative 3.73 (0.35) 6.12 (0.21) 5.85 (0.26)

642

D’ARGEMBEAU ET AL.

7 Non-anxious

Self-Other Bias

Sensorial Details

6

Social Phobics Non-anxious

2

Social Phobics 5 4 3 2

1

0 Positive

1 -1

0 Non-Social

Event Type Figure 1. Mean ratings (/SEM ) for sensorial details of memories for social and non-social events in people with social phobia and non-anxious participants.

information, there was a main effect of event valence, F(1, 32) /6.95, MSE /1.06, p /.01, with other-referential information being better remembered for negative than for positive events. The main effect of group and the group-by-eventvalence interaction were not significant, F (1, 32) / 1.17, MSE /2.82, and F (1, 32) /0.26, MSE /1.06, respectively. To examine whether people with social phobia had a bias towards remembering more selfreferential information than other-referential information, we subtracted ratings for otherreferential information from ratings for self-referential information. This was done separately for positive and negative social events. If the resulting indexes have positive values, this indicates a tendency to remember more self-referential information than other-referential information, whereas negative values indicate the reverse tendency; a value of zero indicates equal amounts of self-referential and other-referential information. Mean values are presented in Figure 2, as a function of group and event valence. A 2 (group) /2 (event valence) ANOVA revealed a main effect of group, F (1, 32) /9.35, MSE /2.33, p/.004, r/.48 (95% CI: .17 to .70). In addition, t tests indicated that the index was significantly higher than zero for people with social phobia, t (16) /3.55, p B/.01, but not for non-anxious participants, t (16) /1.02, p /.32. The valenceby-group interaction was not significant, F(1, 32) /0.81, MSE /1.75. The visual perspective adopted while remembering was analysed with a 2 (group)/2 (event type) /2 (event valence) ANOVA. There was an effect of event type, F (1, 32) /11.40, MSE /2.02, p/.002, which was qualified by a group-by-event-

Event Valence

Figure 2. Mean values (/SEM ) for index of self other bias when remembering positive and negative social events in people with social phobia and non-anxious participants.

type interaction, F(1, 32) /22.12, MSE /2.02, p B/.001 (see Figure 3), and a valence-by-eventtype interaction, F (1, 32) /5.99, MSE /1.96, p / .02. Planned comparisons revealed that people with social phobia remembered social events from an observer perspective more than nonanxious participants did, F (1, 32) /4.98, MSE / 6.25, p/.03, r/.37 (95% CI: .04 to .63), whereas this was not the case for non-social events, F (1, 32) /2.53, MSE /5.96, r / /.27 (95% CI: .56 to .08). The valence-by-event-type interaction revealed that negative social events were remembered from an observer perspective to a greater extent than negative non-social events, F (1, 32)/ 18.27, MSE /1.85, p B/.001, while there was no difference between positive social events and positive non-social events, F(1, 32) / 0.44, MSE /2.13. The three-way interaction was not significant, F (1, 32) /2.53, MSE /1.96. Finally, there was a main effect of group for anxiety ratings, F (1, 32)/6.70, MSE /2.13, p / .01, indicating that, overall, people with social phobia rated the events as involving more anxiety than did non-anxious participants. In addition, 2

Social Phobics Non-anxious

1

Perspective

Social

Negative

0

-1

-2

Social

Non-Social

Event Type Figure 3. Mean ratings (/SEM ) for visual perspective while remembering social and non-social events in people with social phobia and non-anxious participants.

AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY IN SOCIAL PHOBIA

there were main effects of group and event type concerning the age of the events, F (1, 32) /5.77, MSE /19.28, p /.02, and F (1, 32)/7.12, MSE / 6.78, p /.01, respectively, indicating that people with social phobia retrieved more recent events than did non-anxious participants and that non-social events were more recent than social events.

DISCUSSION Previous studies failed to show clear differences in autobiographical memory for social events in social phobia. However, these studies did not assess an essential aspect of autobiographical memory: the subjective experience associated with remembering an event. Accordingly, the purpose of this study was to examine whether phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories for social events are affected by social phobia. People with social phobia and nonanxious participants were asked to remember two social events (one positive, one negative) and two non-social events (one positive, one negative), and to rate the phenomenal characteristics of their memories (i.e., amount of sensorial and contextual detail, amount of self-referential information, amount of information concerning others, visual perspective adopted while remembering). The main findings revealed that the memories of people with social phobia for social events (both positive and negative) contained less sensorial information and more self-referential information than non-anxious participants’ memories. In addition, people with social phobia had a bias towards remembering more self-referential information than other-referential information for social events, whereas this was not the case for non-anxious participants. People with social phobia also remembered social events from an observer perspective to a greater extent than the non-anxious. By contrast, the phenomenal characteristics of the two groups’ memories for nonsocial events did not differ. The finding that the memories of people with social phobia for social events contained more self-referential information is consistent with cognitive models of social phobia (Clark & Wells, 1995; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997). These models hypothesise that when people with social phobia think they are in danger of negative evaluation by others, they shift their attention to detailed monitoring and observation of themselves. This

643

increase in self-focused attention should result in a better encoding of self-referential information in memory. In addition, according to Clark and Wells, when reviewing a social situation they have experienced in the past, people with social phobia predominantly focus on images of themselves in that situation. Such subsequent reactivations of self-referential information should contribute to consolidating this information in memory (Johnson & Chalfonte, 1994). Thus, differences between people with social phobia and nonanxious individuals in terms of memory for selfreferential information may result from differences in both the initial encoding in memory of social events (with people with social phobia focusing more on themselves) and the subsequent reactivation of these events in memory (with people with social phobia reactivating self-referential information to a greater extent). Clark and Wells also proposed that self-focused attention interferes with processing the situation and other people’s behaviour. Consequently, external information (e.g., sensorial details of the situation, contextual information, and information about others) should be less well encoded in memory and thus less well remembered subsequently. Overall, our findings are consistent with this hypothesis: the memories of people with social phobia for social events contained fewer sensorial details than non-anxious individuals’ memories, and people with social phobia had a bias towards remembering more self-referential information relative to other-referential information. However, the two groups did not differ with regard to the amount of contextual detail they remembered about social events, suggesting that not all external information concerning social situations is less well remembered by people with social phobia. It might be that the encoding of these contextual details does not depend on processing resources (i.e., they are encoded automatically in memory; Hasher & Zacks, 1979), and thus they are not reduced in people with social phobia compared to non-anxious individuals. Indeed, there is some evidence indicating that contextual information such as spatial location is encoded automatically in memory (Andrade & Meudell, 1993; Ellis, 1990). Another important aspect of the phenomenology of memory that was assessed in this study pertains to the visual perspective adopted when remembering an event. Initial experiments conducted by Nigro and Neisser (1983) indicated that field and observer perspectives are affected by

644

D’ARGEMBEAU ET AL.

several factors, including the age of the event (with long-ago events being more often remembered from an observer perspective while recent events are more often remembered from a field perspective) and the degree of self-consciousness involved in the event (with situations involving a high degree of self-consciousness, such as giving a public presentation, giving rise to more observer memories than situations involving a low degree of self-consciousness). In addition, confirming previous studies (Coles et al., 2001; Hackmann et al., 1998; Wells et al., 1998; Wells & Papageorgiou, 1999), the present findings indicate that individual differences in social anxiety affect memory perspective, in the case of memory for social events. Previous studies assessed the visual perspective adopted when remembering negative social events compared to non-social events (Wells et al., 1998; Wells & Papageorgiou, 1999) or the visual perspective adopted when remembering social events that varied in their level of anxiety (Coles et al., 2001). To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to examine the influence of the social versus non-social status of the event and its valence simultaneously. We found that both positive and negative social events were more often remembered with an observer perspective by people with social phobia than by non-anxious participants. It is worth noting that these differences in the visual perspective adopted by people with social phobia and non-anxious individuals cannot be explained by differences in the age of the memories recalled. Indeed, the present findings indicate that social events were more often remembered with an observer perspective by people with social phobia despite the fact that these events had occurred more recently than was the case with non-anxious participants (it should be recalled that more recent events are typically remembered with a field perspective; D’Argembeau & Van der Linden, 2004; Nigro & Neisser, 1983). Considering the suggestion that people with social phobia predominantly allocate attentional resources to signs of negative evaluation from others (Clark & Wells, 1995; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997), we predicted that differences between people with social phobia and non-anxious individuals concerning phenomenal characteristics of memory for social events would be particularly marked for negative events. However, our results indicated that the differences between the memories of people with social phobia and non-

anxious participants (with regard to sensorial details, visual perspective, and bias towards remembering self-referential information over other-referential information) were similar for positive and negative social events. Although unexpected, these findings are not particularly surprising when one considers the fact that, in many cases, an event may only be evaluated as positive or negative after it has occurred, or at least once it has begun to unfold. Indeed, one may expect an event to be positive (e.g., going to a party) but this can only be confirmed when the event is experienced. Accordingly, when entering a social situation, individuals with social phobia do not know whether the event will be positive or negative (although they typically expect it to be negative; see e.g., Foa, Franklin, Perry, & Herbert, 1996; Lucock & Salkovskis, 1988) so that some amount of self-focused attention is likely to occur in all cases, resulting in memories for both positive and negative social events that contain more self-referential information and fewer sensorial details and are more often experienced from an observer perspective. Given that people with social phobia had higher BDI-II scores than non-anxious participants in this study, it could be argued that differences in phenomenal characteristics of memory between the two groups may be due to depressive symptoms rather than social phobia. Most studies of memory biases in social phobia (as well as in other anxiety disorders) either did not assess the presence of depressive symptoms or did not address the possible contribution of depressive symptoms to the findings (for reviews, Coles & Heimberg, 2002; Heinrichs & Hofmann, 2001). In other studies, statistical procedures such as analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were used in an attempt to ‘‘control for’’ pre-existing group differences in depressive symptoms (e.g., BDI score). However, it has been argued that this use of an ANCOVA is inappropriate because the covariate and treatment are not statistically independent: briefly, when the covariate and the grouping variable of interest share variance, removing the variance associated with the covariate will also remove some meaningful (shared) variance from the grouping variable of interest, leading to meaningless results (see e.g., Miller & Chapman, 2001, for a discussion of this issue). Indeed, when the covariate and grouping variable are related, the residualised grouping variable (i.e., the group variance that remains after removing the variance associated with the covariate) is

AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY IN SOCIAL PHOBIA

frequently not specifiable in a conceptually meaningful way. For instance, in light of the considerable diagnostic comorbidity of depression and anxiety, Miller and Chapman argued that ‘‘if we compare a sample of depressed patients with nonpatient controls and covary out anxiety, which happens to be higher in the patients, it is not necessarily the case that the residual group difference is a clear, clean representation of depression as it would exist without the comorbid anxiety’’ (2001, p. 43). In the same vein, the fact that there is considerable comorbidity between social phobia and depression (e.g., Kessler, Stang, Wittchen, Stein, & Walters, 1999; Stein, Fuetsch, Muller, Ho¨fler, Lieb, & Wittchen, 2001) suggests that the two conditions are not entirely separate concepts, so that future studies would do well to investigate their joint, and perhaps interactive, effects (e.g., by assessing people with social phobia with varied depression levels) instead of trying to ‘‘control for’’ anything. There is some evidence suggesting that the influence of comorbid depression may in fact differ according to the cognitive processes that are investigated. For instance, Musa et al. (2003) found that attentional biases towards social threat in social phobia were abolished by concurrent depression, whereas Wilson and Rapee (2005) found that interpretative biases associated with social phobia were further increased by comorbid depression. Our findings may in part be due to concurrent depressive symptoms, although it is unlikely that depression completely explains the difference between people with social phobia and nonanxious individuals, because this difference specifically related to memory for social events and it is not clear why depression would have such a specific effect. Nevertheless, this issue should be examined in future studies by comparing groups of people with social phobia with and without comorbid depression. In addition, the present study did not include a control group composed of patients with an anxiety disorder other than social phobia, so it remains to be established that the present findings are specifically related to social phobia, rather than anxiety disorders in general. Finally, it is also worth mentioning that the autobiographical memories that were investigated in this study were recalled intentionally. Memories evoked spontaneously in response to triggers (e.g., when in anxiety-provoking social situations; see Hackmann, Clark, & McManus, 2000) might have different phenomenal charac-

645

teristics, including a higher reliving quality and affective intensity (as is the case for intrusive memories of trauma in post-traumatic stress disorder; Brewin, 2001). It might therefore be the case that the observed differences in phenomenal characteristics between people with social phobia and non-anxious individuals would have been accentuated if the memories were evoked automatically. In addition, one should note that spontaneous images and memories that are experienced by people with social phobia during anxiety-provoking social situations often involve body sensations (e.g., sweating, feeling smaller; Hackmann et al., 2000). Since memory for this type of sensorial details was not assessed in the present study, our finding that people with social phobia experienced fewer sensorial details than non-anxious participants while recalling social events should not be extended to body sensations, which might actually be more prominent in the memories of people with social phobia. Moreover, it would be interesting in future studies to use measures that clearly distinguish between sensorial details concerning the self on the one hand, and sensorial details concerning others and the external environment on the other hand. It might indeed be the case that, when remembering social events, people with social phobia experience fewer sensorial details concerning others and the environment but more sensorial details concerning the self. In conclusion, our findings indicated that the memories of people with social phobia for social events contained fewer sensorial details and more self-referential information than those of nonanxious individuals. In addition, people with social phobia remembered social situations from an observer perspective to a greater extent than the non-anxious people. These differences in memory occurred both for positive and negative social events. By contrast, people with social phobia and non-anxious participants did not differ concerning their memories for positive and negative non-social situations. As suggested by cognitive models of social phobia, people with social phobia may focus their attention on themselves both while experiencing social situations and while reviewing these situations afterwards, thus favouring the encoding and consolidation of self-referential information in memory. Manuscript received 26 April 2005 Manuscript accepted 05 April 2006

646

D’ARGEMBEAU ET AL.

REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Amir, N., Foa, E. B., & Coles, M. E. (1998). Negative interpretation bias in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36 , 945 957. Andrade, J., & Meudell, P. (1993). Is spatial information encoded automatically in memory? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A , 365 375. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for BDI-II . San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Becker, E. S., Rinck, M., Margraf, J., & Roth, W. T. (2001). The emotional Stroop effect in anxiety disorders: General emotionality or disorder specificity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 15 , 147 159. Becker, E. S., Roth, W. T., Andrich, M., & Margraf, J. (1999). Explicit memory in anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108 , 153 163. Breck, B. E., & Smith, S. H. (1983). Selective recall of self-descriptive traits by socially anxious and nonanxious females. Social Behaviour and Personality, 11 , 71 76. Brendle, J. R., & Wenzel, A. (2004). Differentiating between memory and interpretation biases in socially anxious and nonanxious individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42 , 155 171. Brewin, C. R. (2001). A cognitive neuroscience account of posttraumatic stress disorder and its treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39 , 373 393. Clark, D. M., & McManus, F. (2002). Information processing in social phobia. Biological Psychiatry, 51 , 92 100. Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In R. G. Heimberg, M. R. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnostic, assessment and treatment (pp. 69 93). New York: Guilford Press. Cloitre, M., Cancienne, J., Heimberg, R. G., Holt, C. S., & Liebowitz, M. (1995). Memory bias does not generalise across anxiety disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33 , 305 307. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Coles, M. E., & Heimberg, R. G. (2002). Memory biases in the anxiety disorders: Current status. Clinical Psychology Review, 22 , 587 627. Coles, M. E., & Heimberg, R. G. (2005). Recognition bias for critical faces in social phobia: A replication and extension. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43 , 109 120. Coles, M. E., Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., & Fresco, D. M. (2001). Effects of varying levels of anxiety within social situations: Relationship to memory perspective and attributions in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39 , 651 665. Conway, M. A. (2001). Sensory-perceptual episodic memory and its context: Autobiographical memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 356 , 1375 1384.

Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107 , 261 288. Conway, M. A., Singer, J. A., & Tagini, A. (2004). The self and autobiographical memory: Correspondence and coherence. Social Cognition , 22 , 495 537. D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2004). Phenomenal characteristics associated with projecting oneself back into the past and forward into the future: Influence of valence and temporal distance. Consciousness and Cognition , 13 , 844 858. D’Argembeau, A., Van der Linden, M., Etienne, A-M., & Comblain, C. (2003). Identity and expression memory for happy and angry faces in social anxiety. Acta Psychologica , 114 , 1 15. Ellis, N. R. (1990). Is memory for spatial location automatically encoded? Memory & Cognition , 18 , 584 592. Field, A. P., Psychol, C., & Morgan, J. (2004). Postevent processing and the retrieval of autobiographical memories in socially anxious individuals. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18 , 647 663. Foa, E. B., Franklin, M. E., Perry, K. J., & Herbert, J. D. (1996). Cognitive biases in generalised social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105 , 433 439. Foa, E. B., Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Amir, N., & Freshman, M. (2000). Memory bias in generalised social phobia: Remembering negative emotional expressions. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 5 , 501  519. Hackmann, A., Clark, D. M., & McManus, F. (2000). Recurrent images and early memories in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38 , 601  610. Hackmann, A., Surawy, C., & Clark, D. M. (1998). Seeing yourself through other’s eyes: A study of spontaneously occurring images in social phobia. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 26 , 3 12. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 108 , 356 388. Heinrichs, N., & Hofmann, S. G. (2001). Information processing in social phobia: A critical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 21 , 751 770. Hirsch, C. R., Clark, D. M., Mathews, A., & Williams, R. (2003). Self-images play a causal role in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41 , 909  921. Hirsch, C. R., Meynen, T., & Clark, D. M. (2004). Negative self-imagery in social anxiety contaminates social interactions. Memory, 12 , 496 506. Hope, D. A., Rapee, R. M., Heimberg, R. G., & Dombeck, M. J. (1990). Representations of the self in social phobia: Vulnerability to social threat. Cognitive Therapy and Research , 14 , 177 189. Johnson, M. K., & Chalfonte, B. L. (1994). Binding complex memories: The role of reactivation and the hippocampus. In D. L. Schacter & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems (pp. 311 350). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Johnson, M. K., Foley, M. A., Suengas, A. G., & Raye, C. L. (1988). Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical

AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY IN SOCIAL PHOBIA

events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 117 , 371 376. Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin , 114 , 3  28. Kessler, R. C., Stang, P., Wittchen, H-U., Stein, M. B., & Walters, E. E. (1999). Lifetime comorbidities between social phobia and mood disorders in the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine, 29 , 555 567. Lecrubier, Y., Weiller, E., Bonora, L. I., Amorin, P., & Le´pine, J-P. (1994). French adaptation of the Mini International Neuro-psychiatric Interview (MINI 4.4) . Internal report, Unite´ INSERM 302, Hoˆpital de la Salpe´trie`re, Paris, France. Lucock, M. P., & Salkovskis, P. M. (1988). Cognitive factors in social anxiety and its treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 26 , 297 302. ¨ st, L-G. (1996). Recognition bias for Lundh, L-G., & .O critical faces in people with social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34 , 787 794. Mansell, W., & Clark, D. M. (1999). How do I appear to others? Social anxiety and processing of the observable self. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37 , 419 434. Miller, G. A., & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110 , 40 48. Musa, C., Le´pine, J-P., Clark, D. M., Mansell, W., & Ehlers, A. (2003). Selective attention in social phobia and the moderating effect of a concurrent depressive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41 , 1043 1054. Nigro, G., & Neisser, U. (1983). Point of view in personal memories. Cognitive Psychology, 15 , 467 482. O’Banion, K., & Arkowitz, H. (1977). Social anxiety and selective memory for affective information about the self. Social Behaviour and Personality, 5 , 321 328. Pe´rez-Lopez, J. R., & Woody, S. R. (2001). Memory for facial expressions in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39 , 967 975. Rachman, S., Gru¨ter-Andrew, J., & Shafran, R. (2000). Post-event processing in social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38 , 611 617. Rapee, R. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (1997). A cognitivebehavioural model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35 , 741 756. Rapee, R. M., McCallum, S. L., Melville, L. F., Ravenscroft, H., & Rodney, J. M. (1994). Memory bias in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32 , 89 99. Rosnow, R. L., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (2000). Contrasts and correlations in effect-size estimation. Psychological Science, 11 , 446 453.

647

Rubin, D. C., Schrauf, R. W., & Greenberg, D. L. (2003). Belief and recollection of autobiographical memories. Memory & Cognition , 31 , 887 901. Sanz, J. (1996). Memory biases in social anxiety and depression. Cognition and Emotion , 10 , 87 105. Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Harnett-Sheehan, K., Amorin, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59 (suppl. 20), 22 33. Spurr, J. M., & Stopa, L. (2003). The observer perspective: Effects on social anxiety and performance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41 , 1009  1028. Stein, M. B., Fuetsch, M., Muller, N., Ho¨fler, M., Lieb, R., & Wittchen, H-U. (2001). Social anxiety disorder and the risk of depression: A prospective community study of adolescents and young adults. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58 , 251 256. Stopa, L., & Clark, D. M. (2000). Social phobia and interpretation of social events. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38 , 273 283. Suengas, A. G., & Johnson, M. K. (1988). Qualitative effects of rehearsal on memories for perceived and imagined complex events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , 117 , 377 389. Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33 , 448 457. Wells, A., Clark, D. M., & Ahmad, S. (1998). How do I look with my mind’s eye: Perspective taking in social phobic imagery. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36 , 631 634. Wells, A., & Papageorgiou, C. (1999). The observer perspective: Biased imagery in social phobia, agoraphobia, and blood/injury phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37 , 653 658. Wenzel, A., Jackson, L. C., & Holt, C. S. (2002). Social phobia and the recall of autobiographical memories. Depression and Anxiety, 15 , 186 189. Wenzel, A., Werner, M. M., Cochran, C. K., & Holt, C. S. (2004). A differential pattern of autobiographical memory retrieval in social phobic and nonanxious individuals. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 32 , 1 13. Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin , 121 , 331 354. Wilson, J. K., & Rapee, R. M. (2005). The interpretation of negative social events in social phobia with versus without comorbid mood disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19 , 245 274.

Phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories for social ...

Previous studies failed to show clear differences between people with social phobia and non-anxious individuals regarding the specificity and affective intensity of their autobiographical memories for social events. However, these studies did not assess the subjective experience associated with remembering. In this study ...

107KB Sizes 0 Downloads 374 Views

Recommend Documents

Genetic influences on social network characteristics
Feb 4, 2009 - Who becomes the most cen- tral individual in a society and why? What determines how many friends a given individual has? What determines ...

Genetic influences on social network characteristics
Feb 4, 2009 - The heritability of network characteris- tics is important because of its implica- tions for how networks form. Given that social networks play ...

Phenomenal Basis of Epistemic Justification - PhilArchive
consciousness is the basis of epistemic justification and hence that the problem of explaining .... either to phenomenal consciousness or to functional role. In the ...

The Role of Country-of-Origin Characteristics for ...
economic and technological development of the country of origin is supposed to “generate and sustain” (Dunning,. 1979, p. 280) the advantages that specific ...

Characteristics of Echinoderms.pdf
Sign in. Loading… Whoops! There was a problem loading more pages. Retrying... Whoops! There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying.

social-media-map-and-characteristics-poster_0.pdf
Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. social-media-map-and-characteristics-poster_0.pdf. social-media-map-and-characteristics-poster_0.pdf.

Characteristics of the Epic Hero
Basically, this means the hero has the potential for great deeds. ❖ The magnitude of these actions are well above and beyond what the commoner does. ❖ While most epic heroes are good, not all are. Trait 3: Great Warrior. ❖ Before the hero of an

GUIDED-WAVE CHARACTERISTICS OF PERIODICALLY ...
Introduction. The parallel-coupled microstrip line was extensively characterized in the past and has been gaining a wide application in the bandpass filter design ...

experiment_2 study of characteristics of strain ... - MOBILPASAR.COM
CRO. 1. 6. Breadboard. 1. 7. Connecting wires. As required. 8. Probes for CRO. 2. RC PHASE SHIFT OSCILLATOR. PROCEDURE. 1. Connect the components ...

Characteristics of speaking style and implications for ...
form on short-term windows of speech, followed by a non- linear detection operation, and then a second ... the second modulation window size was equal to the length of the spectrogram. The speech segment is .... (Color online) Normalized F0 statistic

Diffusion Characteristics for Simultaneous Source ...
coded signal, or from encrypted signal by making smaller changes in the key. .... [5] John G. Prakis, “Digital Communication”, Mc-Graw Hill Companies, (2007).

2013 Lammers and Proulx Writing autobiographical narratives ...
2013 Lammers and Proulx Writing autobiographical narratives increases political conservatism JESP.pdf. 2013 Lammers and Proulx Writing autobiographical ...

Memories of Micky Edwards.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. Memories of Micky Edwards.pdf. Memories of Micky Edwards.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu.

Characteristics of Business Owners:2002 - Census Bureau
X. X. Majority interest owners. X. X. 48.9 .2. X. X. 39.2 .2. X. X. 63.1 .4. Equal interest owners. X. X. 29.0 .2. X. X. 26.5 .2. X. X. 32.5 .5. Nonmajority interest owners.

Emission characteristics of random lasers
A resonator with an amplifying medium embedded ... an individual spectrum with the ensemble-averaged spectrum that carries the signature of the gain profile.

Characteristics of Business Owners:2002 - Census Bureau
Bureau to take the economic census every 5 years, covering years ending in ''2'' .... the year. Receipts size and employment size are determined for the entire company. ...... lege, but no degree) that the owner(s) completed before establishing, ....

6 Characteristics of Life
6 Characteristics of Life. 1. Reproduction. 2. Grow and Develop. 3. Made of Cells. 4. Respond to a Stimulus. 5. Obtain and Use Energy. 6. Adapt and Evolve ...