PERSPECTIVE ARTICLE published: 25 April 2012 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00112

On discriminating between geometric strategies of surface-based orientation Bradley R. Sturz* and Kent D. Bodily Department of Psychology, Georgia Southern University, Statesboro, GA, USA

Edited by: Ken Cheng, Macquarie University, Australia Reviewed by: Ken Cheng, Macquarie University, Australia *Correspondence: Bradley R. Sturz, Department of Psychology, Georgia Southern University, P.O. Box 8041, Statesboro, GA 30460, USA. e-mail: bradleysturz@ georgiasouthern.edu

Recently, a debate has manifested in the spatial learning literature regarding the shape parameters by which mobile organisms orient with respect to the environment. On one hand are principal-axis-based strategies which suggest that organisms extract the major and minor principal axes of space which pass through the centroid and approximate length and width of the entire space, respectively. On the other hand are medial-axis-based strategies which suggest that organisms extract a trunk-and-branch system similar to the skeleton of a shape. With competing explanations comes the necessity to devise experiments capable of producing divergent predictions. Here, we suggest that a recent experiment (i.e., Sturz and Bodily, 2011a) may be able to shed empirical light on this debate. Specifically, we suggest that a reevaluation of the design reveals that the enclosures used for training and testing appear to produce divergent predictions between these strategies. We suggest that the obtained data appear inconsistent with a medial-axis-based strategy and that the study may provide an example of the types of designs capable of discriminating between these geometric strategies of surface-based orientation. Such an approach appears critical to fundamental issues regarding the nature of space and spatial perception. Keywords: orientation, geometric strategies, medial axis, principal axis

Many mobile organisms that are trained to locate a distinct visual cue in a corner of a rectangular enclosure will respond to an equivalent (above chance) extent to both the trained corner and its 180˚ rotational equivalent (e.g., corners A and B in Figure 1) when the distinct visual cue is removed or rendered useless during subsequent testing (for a review, see Cheng and Newcombe, 2005). This “rotational error” phenomenon has been taken as evidence that organisms incidentally encode information about environmental geometry (Cheng, 1986; Gallistel, 1990). Recently, a debate has manifested in the spatial learning literature regarding the shape parameters by which mobile organisms orient with respect to the environment (Kelly et al., 2011a,b; Sturz and Bodily, 2011b). On the one hand are principal-axis-based strategies which propose that organisms extract the major and minor principal axes of space. The major and minor principal axes pass through the centroid and approximate length and width of the entire space, respectively (for a detailed mathematical and mechanical definition, see Cheng, 2005; see also Cheng and Gallistel, 2005; Bodily et al., 2011; Sturz et al., 2011; left panel, Figure 1). On the other hand are medial-axis-based strategies which propose that organisms extract a trunk-and-branch system similar to the skeleton of a shape (see Blum, 1967; Cheng, 2005; Kelly and Durocher, 2011; Kelly et al., 2011a,b; see right panel, Figure 1). Although the parameters underlying these orientation strategies are geometrically independent, both strategies have been proposed in an effort to account for the ubiquitous rotational error phenomenon described above (for a review, see Cheng, 2005). According to a principal-axis-based strategy (Figure 1, left panel), when organisms learn to approach a distinct cue at location A, they also incidentally learn to search at the right-hand side of www.frontiersin.org

the major principal axis of space (and/or the left-hand side of the minor principal axis of space). When tested in the absence of the distinct cue, a strategy of searching at the right-hand side of the major principal axis (and/or the left-hand side of the minor principal axis) results in equivalent responses to location A and location B (i.e., the rotational error). In contrast, according to a medial-axis-based strategy, when organisms learn to approach a distinct cue at location A, they also incidentally learn to search at the end of the trunk’s terminal right branch. When tested in the absence of the distinct cue, a strategy of searching at the end of the trunk’s terminal right branch results in equivalent responses to location A and location B (i.e., the rotational error). Given these apparent competing explanations of surface-based orientation, it seems critical to devise experiments that are capable of producing divergent predictions – predictions that allow for the discrimination between principal-axis and medial-axis-based strategies of surface-based orientation (see Kelly and Durocher, 2011). In what follows, we reevaluate a recent experiment on geometric orientation strategies (i.e., Sturz and Bodily, 2011a). We suggest that this reevaluation offers a potential example of the types of designs that may be capable of producing divergent predictions between principal-axis and medial-axis-based strategies – designs that may shed empirical light on the this theoretical debate. Recently, we (Sturz and Bodily, 2011a) trained two groups of human participants in a dynamic, three-dimensional virtual environment to respond to two particular corners in hexagon-shaped enclosures [Top Right (TR) and Bottom Left (BL); see Figure 2]. The use of the hexagon-shaped enclosures allowed us to eliminate local geometric cues such as wall lengths and corner angles and isolate global geometric cues for orientation. Importantly, these

April 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 112 | 1

Sturz and Bodily

Geometric strategies

FIGURE 1 | Illustration of principal-axis-based (left panel) and medial-axis-based (right panel) strategies. A represents trained location, and B represents the rotationally equivalent location.

hexagon-shaped enclosures provided participants with enclosures that differed with respect to a hypothetical discriminability ratio between the major and minor principal axes of space. Our rationale was that the discriminability of the principal axis of space (and hence its usefulness as an orienting cue) may be a function of its proportional relationship to the minor principal axis of space. Specifically, we hypothesized that an ability to utilize the major principal axis of space as an orienting cue should increase as it becomes more discriminable from the minor principal axis of space. To that end, we selected two training enclosures for which the major principal axis should be more discriminable from the minor principal axis for one of the enclosures relative to the other (i.e., 0.75 versus 0.25; refer to Figure 2). Subsequent testing in the trained and novel-shaped enclosures revealed that participants trained in the enclosure with the major principal axis hypothesized to be more discriminable (i.e., 0.75) allocated relatively more responses to locations specified by the trained side of the major principal axis of space. We interpreted this obtained difference between the groups as evidence that the discriminability of the major principal axis of space relative to the minor principal axis of space influenced its usefulness as an orienting cue. Although we did not design this experiment to produce divergent predictions between principal-axis and medial-axis-based strategies of surface-based orientation, we suggest that a reevaluation of the design produces unique predictions for each geometric strategy. Specifically, we calculated the medial axis of space for each training and testing enclosure of our experiment (see Figure 3). It is important to note that despite the difference in

Frontiers in Psychology | Comparative Psychology

the enclosure shapes experienced during training by each group, the trunk lengths and terminal-branch lengths were identical in both enclosures. In other words, despite our manipulation of enclosure shape, both groups experienced equivalent training with respect to the medial axis of space. If participants had extracted the medial axis of space during training, then participants in both groups would have been rewarded for searching at the end of the trunk’s terminal right branch. As a result, participants in both groups should have learned an identical medialaxis-based orientation strategy. Consequently, an application of this learned medial-axis-based strategy during the subsequent testing should have resulted in equivalent performance across groups. In short, we suggest that under the conditions of our experiment, a medial-axis-based strategy would predict no difference between the groups during testing because both groups would have relied on identical trunk lengths and terminal-branch lengths. However, obtained results indicated that the groups performed differently during testing. We suggest that such a result appears inconsistent with a medial-axis-based strategy of surface-based orientation. We acknowledge that this retrospective interpretation of our experiment as outlined above is by no means definitive. Specifically, we acknowledge that this interpretation is constrained by numerous differences in potentially critical variables between orientation experiments such as variations in training and testing shapes and/or differences in the species under investigation. In addition, we acknowledge the limitations of basing theoretical

April 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 112 | 2

Sturz and Bodily

Geometric strategies

FIGURE 2 | Schematics of training and testing enclosures of Sturz and Bodily (2011a). Unfilled circles represent invisible response locations, and filled circles represent start locations.

conclusions on statistically null empirical results (as predicted by a medial-axis-based strategy in the present case); however, this statistical limitation could be remedied by recent efforts to utilize Bayesian analyses for determining whether a null hypothesis is supported by the data (see Gallistel, 2009). Despite these limitations, the interpretation provided here may serve as an example of the types of investigations that could discriminate between these strategies of surface-based orientation and may serve as an impetus for adopting a converging evidence approach to illuminating the

www.frontiersin.org

shape parameters by which mobile organisms orient with respect to the environment. Such an approach seems especially critical to remaining issues in comparative spatial cognition regarding perceptible space (i.e., entire space is viewable from a single vantage point) and imperceptible space (i.e., entire space is not viewable from a single vantage point), and it remains unclear the extent to which principalaxis and medial-axis-based strategies are able to account for this kind of partitioning of space into subspaces. Overall, these

April 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 112 | 3

Sturz and Bodily

Geometric strategies

FIGURE 3 | Schematics of training and testing enclosures of Sturz and Bodily (2011a) with the medial axis for each enclosure. Unfilled circles represent invisible response locations, and filled circles represent start locations.

issues appear to reflect larger issues of our objective definitions of space and organisms’ subjective experiences of that space. Collectively, these issues appear to reflect fundamental and unanswered psychophysical questions regarding the nature of space and spatial perception.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by funds from the Office of the Vice President for Research and the Jack N. Averitt College of Graduate Studies at Georgia Southern University.

REFERENCES

Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The Organization of Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Gallistel, C. R. (2009). The importance of proving the null. Psychol. Rev. 116, 439–453. Kelly, D. M., Chiandetti, C., and Vallortigara, G. (2011a). Re-orienting in space: do animals use global or local geometry strategies? Biol. Lett. 7, 372–375. Kelly, D. M., Durocher, S., Chiandetti, C., and Vallortigara, G. (2011b). A misunderstanding of principal and medial axes? Reply to Sturz and Bodily. Biol. Lett. 7, 649–650.

Blum, H. (1967). “A transformation for extracting new descriptors of shape,” in Models for the Perception of Speech and Visual Form, ed. W. WathenDunn (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 362–380. Bodily, K. D., Eastman, C. K., and Sturz, B. R. (2011). Neither by global nor local cues alone: evidence for a unified orientation process. Anim. Cogn. 14, 665–674. Cheng, K. (1986). A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial representation. Cognition 23, 149–178.

Cheng, K. (2005). Reflections on geometry and navigation. Conn. Sci. 17, 5–21. Cheng, K., and Gallistel, C. R. (2005). Shape parameters explain data from spatial transformations: comment on Pearce et al. (2004) and Tommasi and Polli (2004). J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 31, 254–259. Cheng, K., and Newcombe, N. S. (2005). Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 1–23.

Frontiers in Psychology | Comparative Psychology

Kelly, D. M., and Durocher, S. (2011). Comparing geometric models for orientation: medial vs. principal axes. Commun. Integr. Biol. 4, 710–712. Sturz, B. R., and Bodily, K. D. (2011a). Is surface-based orientation influenced by a proportional relationship of shape parameters? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 848–854. Sturz, B. R., and Bodily, K. D. (2011b). Of global space or perceived place? Comment on Kelly et al. Biol. Lett. 7, 647–648. Sturz, B. R., Gurley, T., and Bodily, K. D. (2011). Orientation in

April 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 112 | 4

Sturz and Bodily

Geometric strategies

trapezoid-shaped enclosures: implications for theoretical accounts of geometry learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 37, 246–253.

conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was

Received: 21 March 2012; accepted: 29 March 2012; published online: 25 April 2012.

www.frontiersin.org

Citation: Sturz BR and Bodily KD (2012) On discriminating between geometric strategies of surface-based orientation. Front. Psychology 3:112. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00112 This article was submitted to Frontiers in Comparative Psychology, a specialty of Frontiers in Psychology.

Copyright © 2012 Sturz and Bodily. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial License, which permits non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited.

April 2012 | Volume 3 | Article 112 | 5

On discriminating between geometric strategies of surface ... - Frontiers

Apr 25, 2012 - meters by which mobile organisms orient with respect to the environment. ... human participants in a dynamic, three-dimensional virtual envi-.

1MB Sizes 0 Downloads 241 Views

Recommend Documents

On discriminating between geometric strategies of surface ... - Frontiers
Apr 25, 2012 - Department of Psychology, Georgia Southern University, Statesboro, GA, USA ... GA 30460, USA. ..... March 2012; published online: 25 April.

Coupling of surface plasmons between two silver films ...
gratings2–5 and two-dimensional periodic metal hole array.6–9. Theoretical studies .... m=1, m=2, m=3, and m=4 Ag/SiO2/Ag LSPP modes, which exhibit the ...

EFFECTS OF SURFACE CATALYTICITY ON ...
The risk involved, due to an inadequate knowledge of real gas effects, ... the heat shield surface, increase the overall heat flux up to about two times, or more, higher than ..... using data from wind tunnel and free flight experimental analyses.

The mechanics of embodiment: a dialog on embodiment ... - Frontiers
Jan 31, 2011 - Embodied theories are increasingly challenging traditional views of cognition by arguing that conceptual representations that constitute our knowledge are grounded in sensory and motor experiences, and processed at this sensorimotor le

Inference on Breakdown Frontiers
May 12, 2017 - This paper uses data from phase 1 of SWAY, the Survey of War Affected ...... for all sequences {hn} ⊂ D and {tn} ∈ R+ such that tn ↘ 0, hn − hD ...

Remedial effects of motivational incentive on declining ... - Frontiers
Oct 15, 2010 - Eyetracker PC-60 (Version 2.7, Arrington Research Inc.3) software on a standard PC. Bidirectional ..... Heekeren, H. R. (2005). Reward-based.

Remedial effects of motivational incentive on declining ... - Frontiers
Oct 15, 2010 - dial effects may in fact be unobservable. To investigate the remedial ...... 22 September 2010; published online: 15. October 2010. Citation: ...

Opposite effects of visual and auditory word-likeness on ... - Frontiers
Aug 29, 2013 - The present fMRI study investigated the effects of word-likeness of visual and auditory stimuli on activity along the ventral visual stream. In the context of a one-back task, we presented visual and auditory words, pseudowords, and ar

Opposite effects of visual and auditory word-likeness on ... - Frontiers
Aug 29, 2013 - auditory processing are expected in visual regions. Keywords: fMRI .... million (based on the CELEX database; Baayen et al., 1993). The pseudowords matched the ...... an open-access article distributed under the terms of the.

Measurement of vibrations induced on the surface of crystalline eye ...
aDepartment of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Houston, ... cDepartment of Biomedical Engineering, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, ...

ON LOVELY PAIRS OF GEOMETRIC STRUCTURES 1 ...
of rank ≤ ω. The tools used in the proof depended mainly on the description of definable sets given by van den Dries in [10]. Since such a description can be.

Correlations between Surface Temperature Trends and ...
modeled data are completely unlike those on the observed data. Table 4 shows that the magnitudes are typically one-tenth those of the observed coefficients.

A. Szatkowski - On geometric formulation of dynamical systems. Parts I ...
of hidden constraints by analysing the description of the constitutive space of the system. Hidden. constraints .... a tangent vector to the space .... Parts I and II.pdf.

A. Szatkowski - On geometric formulation of dynamical systems. Parts I ...
Page 3 of 86. A. Szatkowski - On geometric formulation of dynamical systems. Parts I and II.pdf. A. Szatkowski - On geometric formulation of dynamical systems.

Proceedins of the Symposium on Geometric Function ...
Proceedins of the Symposium on. Geometric Function Theory. Organisors. A Gangadharan, V Ravichandran. Editors. V Ravichandran, N Marikkannan. Sri Venkateswara College of Engineering. Pennalur, Sriperumbudur 602 105, Tamil Nadu. December 2, 2002 ...

Measurement of vibrations induced on the surface of crystalline eye ...
line scan camera (Basler L104K-2k, having 2048 pixel resolution, line rate of 29.2 ..... speaker diaphragm surface at without vibration. Frequency (Hz). D s ip la.

TMII FRONTIERS OF -
Abstract: The intrinsic motion sensitivity of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) can be used acquire and quantify blood flow in-vivo. Advances in imaging ...

Temporal evolution of surface ripples on a finite ... - EGR.MSU.Edu
we integrate analytic solutions for the temporal evolution of the initial ripples on ...... F. Garanin, V. A. Ivanov, V. P. Korchagin, O. D. Mikhailov, I. V.. Morozov, S. V. ...

Excitation of surface dipole and solenoidal modes on toroidal structures
May 1, 2006 - ... field, microwave radiation. ∗Electronic address: [email protected]. 1 ... the basis set are given and the solution method detailed. Section 4 presents results in the ..... 13, wherein a clear signature of a circulatory Jb.

Modeling reaction-diffusion of molecules on surface and ... - CiteSeerX
MinE and comparing their simulated localization patterns to the observations in ..... Hutchison, “E-CELL: software environment for whole-cell simulation,” ... 87–127. [11] J. Elf and M. Ehrenberg, “Spontaneous separation of bi-stable biochem-

Effect of surface termination on the electronic properties ...
estimate the amount of accumulated charge for both terminations by computing the electron occupation of Löwdin atomic orbitals [6] on each atom in the fully ...

Compositional Variation on the Surface of Centaur ...
Subject headings: infrared: solar system — Kuiper Belt, Oort Cloud. 1. INTRODUCTION ... heliocentric distances in the solar nebula. These distant objects.

Rapid comparison of properties on protein surface
Jul 10, 2008 - 4 Markey Center for Structural Biology, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana 47907. 5 The Bindley ... per proteins, and proteins in the ubiquitination pathway.10–14 ... several protein families including globins, thermo-.

Modeling reaction-diffusion of molecules on surface ...
Abstract—The E-Cell System is an advanced open-source simulation platform to model and analyze biochemical reaction networks. The present algorithm ...