Near to the Brain: Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy as a Lightweight Brain Imaging Technique for Visualization Evan M. Peck

Erin Treacy Solovey

Sara Su

Robert J.K. Jacob

Remco Chang

Tufts University [email protected]

Tufts University [email protected]

Google Inc. [email protected]

Tufts University [email protected]

Tufts University [email protected]

0.4

0.5

diceret consulatu consetetur vel.

0.2 0 diceret consulatu consetetur vel.

0 −0.2

−0.5

diceret consulatu consetetur vel.

−0.4 0 Neg. Contrast Polarity

Small Contrast

10

20

30

Pos. Contrast Polarity

0

20 Bar Graph

40 Pie Chart

Figure 1: (Left) Activation in the prefrontal cortex while subjects performed a visual search task under different brightness contrast conditions. The brain activity plot is the mean change in deoxygenated hemoglobin over 130 trials of each condition (13 participants, 10 trials of each condition). The x-axis represents time (in seconds) over the course of a single trial. The y-axis is in micromolars and roughly represents activation levels. Finally, the width of the line represents standard error at each time point. (Right) Activation levels during angle and position judgment tasks in pie charts and bar graphs. The brain activity plot is the mean chance in deoxygenated hemoglobin over 36 trials of each condition (3 participants, 12 trials of each condition).

A BSTRACT In order to better understand the user and visual interface, it is crucial to also understand human cognitive processes. Unfortunately, these processes are traditionally difficult to monitor without the use of cumbersome or expensive brain imaging equipment. In recent years, functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) has emerged as a brain imaging technique that is both lightweight and easy to set up. In this paper, we demonstrate the potential of fNIRS to examine current visualization techniques and influence the design of visual interfaces. To validate fNIRS as a tool for visualization research, we present two studies based on previous work in brightness contrast in visual search and angle vs. position comparisons in form. Our results indicate there are significant and unintuitive cognitive differences in the prefrontal cortex during visual search tasks of positive and negative contrast polarity. Furthermore, we are able to differentiate between angle and position comparisons under specific experimental conditions. Finally, we outline the potential of fNIRS to give objective, continuous, and near real-time feedback of brain activity in future visualization research. 1 I NTRODUCTION Understanding human perceptual and cognitive processes has long been acknowledged by researchers in visualization and visual analytics as a core mission of the community. If one can observe a user’s mental processes while viewing and interacting with a visualization, one can better evaluate the efficacy of visual interfaces,

design new visual representations, and develop novel interaction techniques. Unfortunately, observing a subject’s cognitive processes when using a visualization has proven difficult. Use of successful brain sensing technologies such as electroencephalography (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been limited due to cost, cumbersome set-up, or movement artifacts that interfere with the signal. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) is an emerging technology for brain imaging being developed as an alternative to EEG and fMRI. It is lightweight, low-cost, non-invasive, easy to set up, and relatively robust with respect to movement artifacts. fNIRS uses near-infrared light to measure concentration and oxygenation of blood in the tissue at depths of 1-3cm [7]. Light is injected into the forehead in the near infrared range (650-900 nm), where it is diffusely reflected by the scalp, skull, and brain cortex. By measuring the light returned to the detector, researchers are able to calculate the amount of oxygen in the blood, as well as the amount of blood in the tissue. Since changes in blood flow and oxygenation indicate activation levels in the brain, much like fMRI, we can use fNIRS to measure activity in localized areas of the brain. As a result, fNIRS has increasingly been used in the humancomputer interaction (HCI) community for studying cognitive processes in real-world environments. Researchers have used fNIRS to identify verbal and spatial working memory [2], preference [3], emotion [6], interruption [5], and response inhibition [2]. We chose to replicate perceptual studies with quantifiable results to explore the fitness of fNIRS in a visualization context. By comparing our results with the known results of the studies, we believe that it provides a baseline for potential research with fNIRS and lends credibility to more exploratory studies in the future.

Figure 2: (Left) An fNIRS probe with five light sources and one detector. (Right) A headband holds two fNIRS probes in place on a study participant’s forehead.

2 S TUDY 1: C ONTRAST AND V ISUAL S EARCH Studies suggest that increasing the contrast in first-order perceptual features improves performance on search tasks [4]. As an example, light text on a dark background is more difficult to read than dark text on a light background and the minimum luminance contrast between foreground and background color should be about 27 units (CIELAB lightness) [8]. Because contrast and visual search have such widespread implications in visualization, we believe that it is a prime research area to test the experimental validity of fNIRS in visualization research. Following work by Zuffi et al., we used a visual search task in which participants search for occurrences of a character within a body of text (in this case, the letter ‘e’). Text and background colors were selected for specific differences in brightness levels, using the HSB color model, to create three contrast conditions for visual search: small contrast, large contrast with positive polarity, and large contrast with negative polarity. • Hypothesis: fNIRS can measure differences in visual search between the small and large brightness contrast conditions, as well as the positive and negative contrast polarity conditions. During the course of one 30 second trial, participants saw 10 phrases of pseudo-text, each for a 3 second interval. The phrases, which consisted of 5 words each, were randomly selected from a body of lorem ipsum text. Participants were asked to visually scan each phrase as it was presented to them and press the space bar on the keyboard each time they saw the letter ‘e’. Participants were shown 10 trials of each condition. Our results, demonstrated in Figure 1, indicate that there are measurable differences in activation in the prefrontal cortex between the three conditions. Although these differences were expected, it is surprising to observe that the cognitive footprints vary most dramatically between positive and negative polarity conditions, with lesser distinction between small and large contrast levels. It is important to note that it is difficult to compare task workloads exclusively from looking at fNIRS readings. We can make observations about activation in the prefrontal cortex, but that activation might not correlate to overall workload in the brain. 3 S TUDY 2: F ORM AND S PATIAL W ORKING M EMORY In their landmark paper, Cleveland and McGill [1] demonstrated that judgment tasks relying on position along a common scale are more accurate than judgment tasks that rely on angle. Their experiment, based on comparisons in bar graphs and pie charts, created a baseline for visualization work in the study of form. • Hypothesis: fNIRS can measure differences in spatial working memory between percentage judgments in bar graphs and in pie charts. In our version of Cleveland and McGill’s experiment, participants were shown slides with a pie chart or bar graph every 3.75 seconds. However, instead of making percentage estimates of a smaller graph

section (marked by a red dot) to a larger graph section (marked by a black dot) in the current slide, participants compared a smaller section in the current slide, to a larger section in the previous slide. This variation of the 1-back task - a test used by psychologists to induce working memory - requires participants to maintain the mental image of graph sections between slides. By taxing their spatial working memory, it gives us a better opportunity to observe differences in activation between conditions. Each trial lasted 45 seconds and showed 12 slides, resulting in 12 comparisons over the course of the trial. Participants were shown 12 trials of each condition pie chart and bar graph. Early results indicate that our hypothesis holds. By looking at Figure 1, we can see that there are significant differences in fNIRS signals during participant interaction with bar graphs vs. pie charts that are likely caused by varying loads on spatial working memory. 4 C ONCLUSION AND D ISCUSSION In the first study, we used fNIRS to capture contrast differences in visual search conditions. We found differences between negative contrast polarity and positive contrast polarity which were significantly more striking than difference in high contrast and low contrast conditions. These results raise interesting questions about the impact of contrast polarity in visualizations. Are there cognitive effects that could impact a user’s experience with a visualization, either positive or negative, that have not been aptly captured in behavioral studies? In addition, we believe that this study can be extended to investigate other visualization questions about optimal design for legibility, visual search, and reduction of visual clutter. In the second study, we saw evidence that we can distinguish between angle and position judgments in bar graphs and pie charts. We believe that similar setups can be used to examine interactions in more complex graphical layouts and forms where behavioral measures may be difficult to extract. Based on our findings, we propose that fNIRS can offer similar benefits to visualization researchers and practitioners as those experienced by the HCI community. fNIRS can give passive information about users as they perceive, cognitively digest, subjectively experience, and interact with visualizations. R EFERENCES [1] W. S. Cleveland and R. McGill. Graphical Perception: Theory, Experimentation, and Application to the Development of Graphical Methods. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 79(387):531, Sept. 1984. [2] L. M. Hirshfield, R. Gulotta, S. Hirshfield, S. Hincks, M. Russel, R. Ward, T. Williams, and R. J. K. Jacob. This is Your Brain on Interfaces : Enhancing Usability Testing with Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy. In CHI 2011, 2011. [3] S. Luu and T. Chau. Decoding subjective preference from single-trial near-infrared spectroscopy signals. Journal of Neural Engineering, 6, 2008. [4] J. Robson. Spatial and Temporal Contrast-Sensitivity Functions of the Visual System. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 56(8):1141– 1142, 1966. [5] E. T. Solovey, F. Lalooses, K. Chauncey, D. Weaver, M. Scheutz, A. Sassaroli, S. Fantini, P. Schermerhorn, and R. J. K. Jacob. Sensing Cognitive Multitasking for a Brain-Based Adaptive User Interface. In CHI 2011, 2011. [6] K. Tai and T. Chau. Single-trial classification of NIRS signals during emotional induction tasks: towards a corporeal machine interface. Journal of neuroengineering and rehabilitation, 6:39, Jan. 2009. [7] a. Villringer and B. Chance. Non-invasive optical spectroscopy and imaging of human brain function. Trends in neurosciences, 20(10):435– 42, Oct. 1997. [8] S. Zuffi, C. Brambilla, G. Beretta, and P. Scala. Human Computer Interaction: Legibility and Contrast. In 14th International Conference on Image Analysis and Processing (ICIAP 2007), number Iciap, pages 241–246. Ieee, Sept. 2007.

Near to the Brain: Functional Near-Infrared ... - Research at Google

Lightweight Brain Imaging Technique for Visualization ... To validate fNIRS as a tool for visualization research, .... R. Ward, T. Williams, and R. J. K. Jacob. This is ...

310KB Sizes 0 Downloads 246 Views

Recommend Documents

Near Neighbor Join - Research at Google
recommendations, social networking sites routinely compare the behaviors of ... In the following Top-Down (TD) phase, at each iteration the most similar pairs of representatives are selected to guide the comparison of objects they ..... 10: from = pa

“Near-Duplicates”: Learning Hash Codes for ... - Research at Google
applications, including search-by-example on large ... Retrieval uses the same banding ..... Applications”, Data Mining and Knowledge Discovery, 2008.

Near Impressions for Observational Causal Ad ... - Research at Google
... the performance of their online advertising by either running ran- ..... sure the impact of their display and video ad ..... iment” Journal of Educational Psychology.

Mesa: Geo-Replicated, Near Real-Time ... - Research at Google
Jan 1, 2014 - [10] have proposed the MaSM (materialized sort-merge) algorithm, which can be used ... lems of data corruption that may result from software errors and hardware ...... other companies that need to be supported. In summary,.

Functional brain mapping of psychopathology - JNNP
first degree relative with psychosis requiring admission to hospital) ..... Psychol Med 2000;30:491–504. ..... detection, and the online monitoring of performance.

Accuracy at the Top - Research at Google
We define an algorithm optimizing a convex surrogate of the ... as search engines or recommendation systems, since most users of these systems browse or ...

Functional brain mapping of psychopathology - JNNP
models of psychological function and to express these in terms of patterns ..... Am J. Psychiatry 1987;144:151–8. 74 Zemishlany Z, Alexander GE, Prohovnik I, et al. Cortical blood ... detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science.

Whole brain functional connectivity in the early blind
May 28, 2007 - 2Department of Radiology, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital University of Medical Sciences, ..... AAL¼ Automated Anatomical Labeling. P age4o f12. Brain. (2007). Y. L iu ..... language-related working memory during online sentence.

Improving Access to Web Content at Google - Research at Google
Mar 12, 2008 - No Javascript. • Supports older and newer browsers alike. Lynx anyone? • Access keys; section headers. • Labels, filters, multi-account support ... my screen- reading application, this site is completely accessible for people wit

Mathematics at - Research at Google
Index. 1. How Google started. 2. PageRank. 3. Gallery of Mathematics. 4. Questions ... http://www.google.es/intl/es/about/corporate/company/history.html. ○.

Migrating to BeyondCorp - Research at Google
involved, from the teams that own individual services, to management, to support teams, to ... a new network design, one that removes the privilege of direct.

GRAPHEME-TO-PHONEME CONVERSION ... - Research at Google
model and the performance of the overall system relies on the coverage and quality of .... knowledge is the first such application of LSTMs. 4. LSTM-BASED G2P ...

Google's Hybrid Approach to Research - Research at Google
To compare our approach to research with that of other companies is beyond the scope of this paper. ... plores fundamental research ideas, develops and maintains the software, and helps .... [8], Google File System [9] and BigTable [10]. 2.

Mobile Computing: Looking to the Future - Research at Google
May 29, 2011 - Page 1 ... fast wired and wireless networks make it economical ... ple access information and use network services. Bill N. Schilit, Google.

RLint: Reformatting R Code to Follow the ... - Research at Google
Jul 2, 2014 - Improves programmer productivity. Suggest ... R programmer time is expensive? .... Application: Improve R community's style consistency.

Using Machine Learning to Improve the Email ... - Research at Google
Using Machine Learning to Improve the Email Experience ... services including email, and machine learning has come of ... Smart Reply: Automated response.

Learning to Rank Recommendations with the k ... - Research at Google
optimize precision at k than the WARP loss, which WARP was designed for. Secondly, it can optimize novel metrics like maximum mean rank which we believe ...

Using the Wave Protocol to Represent ... - Research at Google
There are several challenges in aggregating health records from multiple sources, including merging data, preserving proper attribution, and allowing.

Projecting the Knowledge Graph to Syntactic ... - Research at Google
lation; for example, the name of a book, it's author, other books ... Of the many publicly available KBs, we focus this study ... parse tree in the search space that is “not worse” than y. .... the parser accuracy in labelling out-of-domain en- t

Introduction to the Aggregate Marketing System ... - Research at Google
Apr 13, 2017 - 2015), and geo experiments (Vaver & Koehler, 2011), across complex modeling scenarios. ... Measuring media effects across many channels, for example, requires ...... In branded search campaigns, the advertiser benefits from a competiti

From Freebase to Wikidata: The Great Migration - Research at Google
include pages from online social network sites, reviews on shopping sites, file .... more than ten facts—in the center of the Figure—has been mapped. The items ...

Faucet - Research at Google
infrastructure, allowing new network services and bug fixes to be rapidly and safely .... as shown in figure 1, realizing the benefits of SDN in that network without ...

BeyondCorp - Research at Google
41, NO. 1 www.usenix.org. BeyondCorp. Design to Deployment at Google ... internal networks and external networks to be completely untrusted, and ... the Trust Inferer, Device Inventory Service, Access Control Engine, Access Policy, Gate-.