ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2008, 75, 1981e1987 doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.12.008

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

Large body size for winning and large swords for winning quickly in swordtail males, Xiphophorus helleri JOHN PRENTER*†, PHILLIP W. TAYLOR † & ROBERT W . EL WOOD*

*School of Biological Sciences, Queen’s University Belfast yCentre for the Integrative Study of Animal Behaviour, Macquarie University (Received 18 July 2007; initial acceptance 17 September 2007; final acceptance 25 December 2007; published online 16 April 2008; MS. number: 9460)

Contestants can either assess their own resource-holding potential relative to their opponent (mutual assessment) or rely solely on the assessment of their own fighting ability (self-assessment). To discriminate between these possibilities, we staged dyadic territorial contests between ‘size-matched’ male swordtails. These contests consist of a combination of ritualized displays and direct fighting. Although size differences were small, winners were larger than losers and smaller fish tended to be winners only when the size difference was negligible. Body size, however, did not influence contest duration and there was no increase in contest duration with mean body size; thus, there is no support for self-assessment in these animals. We also examined the effects of the sword, which comprises a sexually selected extension used in female choice that reduces swimming efficiency but increases acceleration. The length of the sword (adjusted for body size) did not differ between winners and losers; however, losers conceded earlier if the opponent had a large sword for its body size but this decision was independent of the loser’s own sword length. Losers thus assessed the swords of winners, which precludes self-assessment; however, because winners appeared not to assess the swords of losers, this does not fully support the idea of mutual assessment. ! 2008 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: honest signal; maleemale contest; mutual assessment; self-assessment; swordtail fish; Xiphophorus helleri

Animal contests are usually settled on the basis of asymmetries in fighting ability or resource-holding potential (RHP; reviewed in Huntingford & Turner 1987; Andersson 1994; Riechert 1998; Briffa & Sneddon 2007), resource value (e.g. Cross et al. 2006) and resource ownership (Olsson & Shine 2000). Previous experience (reviewed in Hsu et al. 2006), injuries (Taylor & Jackson 2003), metabolic costs and/or constraints (reviewed in Briffa & Sneddon 2007) and hunger (Stocker & Huber 2001) may influence contest outcome. Selection is expected to favour individuals that gather accurate information about the Correspondence: J. Prenter, Centre for the Integrative Study of Animal Behaviour, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia (email: [email protected]). P. W. Taylor is at the Centre for the Integrative Study of Animal behaviour, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia. R. W. Elwood is at the School of Biological Sciences, Queen’s University Belfast, Medical Biology Centre, 97 Lisburn Road, Belfast BT9 7BL, U.K. 0003e 3472/08/$34.00/0

costs and benefits of conflict (Parker 1974; Maynard Smith & Parker 1976; Parker & Rubenstein 1981). Contestants with information about their own RHP are expected to benefit if they can also assess the ability of their opponents. Such ‘mutual assessment’ allows rivals to obtain a reliable estimate of differences in ability between themselves and their opponent. They can then use this information in deciding whether to continue fighting. Alternatively, contestants may rely solely on an estimate of their own RHP, persisting in contests in accordance with their own fighting ability (‘self-assessment’ or ‘own size assessment’; e.g. Taylor & Elwood 2003), and not in accordance with their relative RHP. In both cases, the loser’s decision to give up determines the duration of the contest. A negative relation is commonly found between the relative size of the contestants and the cost (duration, physiological change or number of specific signals) of a contest and this trend has historically been considered as support for the hypothesis of mutual assessment.

1981 ! 2008 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1982

ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 75, 6

However, self-assessment may also produce this relation (Taylor & Elwood 2003). These two hypotheses may be distinguished by two main methods. First, in random pairings, the relation between the winner’s size and the costs of the contest is expected to be positive in self-assessment but negative for mutual assessment (Taylor et al. 2001; Taylor & Elwood 2003). Second, large size-matched pairs are expected to incur greater costs than small size-matched pairs in self-assessment but not in mutual assessment (Taylor & Elwood 2003). We investigated the mode of assessment and contest resolution in swordtail fish (Poeciliidae). Competitive interactions between males are well documented (e.g. Francis 1983; Franck & Ribowski 1987, 1989; Beaugrand et al. 1991, 1996), comprising a series of escalating activities (see Franck & Ribowski 1989) from a form of lateral inspection and display to biting, nipping and mouth wrestling. Previous work has largely focused on factors influencing contest outcome although, as with much research on contests (Taylor & Elwood 2003), until recently the prospect of self-assessment has been largely ignored. Body size, sword length, previous experience (but see Earley & Dugatkin 2002) and prior residency have all been reported to affect (either individually or in combination) contest outcome in swordtails (Hemens 1966; Franck & Hendricks 1973; Franck & Ribowski 1987, 1989; Beaugrand & Beaugrand 1991; Beaugrand et al. 1991, 1996; Beaugrand 1997; Benson & Basolo 2006). The ‘sword’ in these fish comprises a set of multicoloured fin rays extending well beyond the caudal margin (Benson & Basolo 2006). Large swords confer an advantage in attracting females (Basolo 1990; Basolo & Trainor 2002) but at a cost to the bearer in terms of predation risk (Basolo & Wagner 2004) and increased energy expenditure in sustained swimming (Basolo & Alcaraz 2003). However, large swords enhance the fast-start swimming response that, in contradiction to the above, should increase ability to escape from predators (Royle et al. 2006). Because fights are generally energetically demanding (Briffa & Sneddon 2007), it is possible that males would suffer a cost of long swords in contests. However, Benson & Basolo (2006) found that variation in natural sword length did not influence the outcome of fights. When artificial swords of equal size were attached to fish but painted to appear different, males with the apparently larger swords were more likely to win (Benson & Basolo 2006). In this case the cost of the apparently large and small swords would have been the same because they would have had the same hydrodynamic effect. We explored how variation in sword length influences fight outcome and fight duration and whether that might help distinguish between self- and mutual assessment. By examining contests between size-matched rivals, we should be better able to investigate the effects of sword length on contests. If longer swords impose a cost in contests, then for selfassessment we predicted that losers would have longer swords for their body size than winners and that the longer the sword the sooner the animal should give up. For mutual assessment we predicted that relative sword length would be a determinant of contest duration with

swift resolution occurring when the winners have swords that are much shorter than those of losers. Alternatively, the sword might confer an advantage (Benson & Basolo 2006) and this could be for two reasons. First, the sword might specifically enhance the ability to fight (e.g. by increasing stability during slow movements) and either self-assessment or mutual assessment could occur. Second, it might be a signal to the opponent that the bearer is in good condition and therefore a good fighter, but without specifically enhancing fighting ability, that is, it might be a badge (e.g. Rohwer 1975; reviewed in Searcy & Nowicki 2005; but see Royle et al. 2005). In this case the sword can only be used to influence the motivation of the opponent and that implies mutual assessment. Thus if the relation between sword length and contest duration indicates self-assessment, it would also indicate that the sword confers an advantage in RHP. If this relation indicates mutual assessment, the sword could either increase RHP or be a signal of quality. METHODS

General Methods Swordtail males were obtained from a local supplier (Grosvenor Tropicals, Belfast, U.K.) and kept in mixed-sex groups in laboratory aquaria ranging in size from 54 to 90 litres. Tanks were equipped with chemical and biological filtration, substantial aeration and a gravel substrate to a depth of 6 cm with plants as refuges. Temperature was maintained between 25 and 27! C and pH between 7 and 8, with a 12:12 h light:dark cycle. Fish were fed Tetra-Min flake food (Tetra GmbH, Melle, Germany) twice daily, and bloodworm (red mosquito larvae, San Francisco Bay Brand, Newark, California, U.S.A.) once a day. Two groups of males were maintained separately with females. Allowing males to be exposed to females reduced the risk of inappropriate responses to conspecifics (Franck & Hendricks 1973) and was expected to promote competition between males. Our basic methodology followed that of Earley et al. (2005). We removed males from holding tanks, transferred them to individual 8-litre screened-off, isolation tanks for 48 h, and then placed them in experimental aquaria for a 16 h acclimation period. This gives a total period of 64 h without social interaction to mitigate the behavioural effects of prior experience of winning and losing that may persist for at least 24 h in X. helleri (Franck & Ribowski 1987). Experimental aquaria consisted of a 20-litre tank divided in half by an opaque, removable, corregated plastic partition. The partition was removed at the start of an observation. All pairs were novel and matched by eye, resulting in a mean size difference of 5.83% body length. This resulted in a strong correlation between the size of the larger and smaller opponents in the dyads (Pearson correlation: r29 ¼ 0.86, P < 0.0001), but a weaker relation between the swords of the contestants (r29 ¼ 0.41, P ¼ 0.021). Each male of the pair was from a different stock tank. Experimental tanks, isolation tanks, stock tanks and subjects in experimental tanks were visually isolated from one another, thereby

PRENTER ET AL.: MALEe MALE CONTESTS IN SWORDTAIL FISH

eliminating the possibility of gaining information by ‘eavesdropping’ (Earley & Dugatkin 2002). Determining the point in the interaction at which the contest is over and the outcome is decided is key to our analysis. We defined the decision time (end of a contest) as the initiation of chasing. The initiation of chasing by one contestant and rapid retreat by the other denotes the point when contest roles are decided (fixed; see Benson & Basolo 2006). Contest duration was determined as the interval between the start of the interaction and the initiation of chasing behaviour.

Morphological Measurements Immediately after observation, we removed both fish from the experimental tanks using a hand net and killed them with a lethal dose of the anaesthetic MS222. We measured males with digital callipers (Moore & Wright model no. M&W3412280B). For each fish we measured body length (mm) from the anterior of the head to the base of the caudal fin along the mid line, body (flank) height (mm) from gonopore to base of dorsal fin, and sword length (mm) from the proximal margin of the caudal fin to the distal tip of the sword. We estimated the body surface area as the area of an ellipse (area ¼ p # (a # b), where a is the length of the major axis/2, i.e. body length/2, and b ¼ the length of the minor axis/2, i.e. body (flank) height/2) as a measure of body size that is independent of the sword length.

Statistical Analyses Two-tailed summary statistics are presented as mean $ SE. For analyses we used JMP version 5 for Macintosh (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, U.S.A.). Prior to carrying out tests, we checked the normality of data by visual examination of distributions and by a ShapiroeWilks W test. Non-normally distributed data were log transformed before parametric analyses were done. Body size and sword length in winners and losers were compared in paired t tests. We also examined how size and sword length predicted outcome in a multivariate logistic regression. Univariate and multivariate regression analyses were used to examine relations between contest cost (duration and intensity, i.e. level of contest escalation) and RHP (fighting ability).

Ethical Note In nature, swordtail males defend home ranges aggressively against other males (Franck & Ribowski 1993; Franck et al. 1998). While it was our intention to intervene should levels of aggression become so severe as to result in physical injury, this was not necessary. We did not detect any wounds or mortality as a result of observed aggressive behaviour in test subjects. Males were always free to escape an aggressor by swimming away to another area of the tank. All aspects of the research were covered by a U.K. Home Office project licence and conducted by an

experienced personal licence holder. Subject males were killed after observation to provide data for another study. RESULTS Thirty-seven maleemale contests were observed. However, data on contest duration are available for only 31 dyads, as chasing did not occur in six pairings (i.e. the contest was not resolved before termination of observation, 1 h). Mean contest duration $ SE was 1000.27 $ 122.09 s (range 155.2e2951.7 s). Sword length was weakly correlated with body length (Pearson correlation: r60 ¼ 0.261, P ¼ 0.040), but not with body height (r60 ¼ 0.020, P ¼ 0.880) or body surface area (r60 ¼ 0.139, P ¼ 0.283). Winning males had longer and taller bodies, and consequently greater body surface area than losers (Table 1). However, we found no statistical differences between winners and losers with regard to absolute sword length or residuals of sword length against body size (size-corrected sword length). We therefore used body surface area as a measure of body size in further analyses. A multivariate logistic regression examined which variables predicted outcome in these contests when examined in combination. An overall model including body size for the larger and smaller animal and body size-corrected sword length for each male (residuals) was not significant (U ¼ 0.21, N ¼ 31, c24 ¼ 7:26, P ¼ 0.108), although the size of the larger (likelihood ratio test: G1 ¼ 4.84, P ¼ 0.028, B ¼ %12.49) and smaller male (G1 ¼ 5.96, P ¼ 0.015, B ¼ 13.57) had equal and opposite effects on the probability that the larger contestant won. Body size-corrected sword length did not predict contest outcome (larger male: G1 ¼ 0.46, P ¼ 0.499, B ¼ %0.08; smaller male: G1 ¼ 0.34, P ¼ 0.562, B ¼ %0.09). To examine the effects of body size difference on the probability of the larger fish winning, we repeated the analysis but used body size difference instead of the individual sizes. The overall model was marginally nonsignificant (overall model: U ¼ 0.21, N ¼ 31, c23 ¼ 7:26P ¼ 0.064). However, size difference predicted outcome in terms of whether or not the larger contestant won (G1 ¼ 5.64, P ¼ 0.018, Table 1. Comparison of morphological measurements between winners and losers in contests between male swordtails, X. helleri, matched for size Loser

Winner

t30

P

Body length 44.85$1.09 46.84$1.01 %3.22 0.003 (mm) Body height 13.38$0.36 14.06$0.35 %2.98 0.006 (mm) Surface area 478.92$24.02 523.50$22.71 %3.58 0.001 (mm2) Sword length 31.30$1.08 29.92$0.89 0.69 0.495 (mm) Size-corrected 0.02$0.04 %0.02$0.03 0.88 0.385 sword length (mm) Means $ SE for raw (untransformed) data are presented; paired t tests were conducted on transformed data (see text for details of calculations).

1983

ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 75, 6

B ¼ %12.75), that is, the more disparate in size the greater the chance of the larger animal winning. Residuals of sword length did not predict contest outcome (larger male: G1 ¼ 0.42, P ¼ 0.518, B ¼ %0.07; smaller male: G1 ¼ 0.59, P ¼ 0.443, B ¼ %0.11). Thus, body size but not sword length influenced the outcome of fights.

RHP, Contest Duration and Escalation We carried out a multivariate regression on log contest duration with body size and body size-corrected sword length for winners and losers. The overall model was not significant (N ¼ 31, F4,30 ¼ 2.23, R2 ¼ 0.256, P ¼ 0.093). However, size-corrected sword length for winners was negatively related to contest duration (F1,30 ¼ 6.65, b ¼ %2.30, P ¼ 0.016) but no other variable was significant (body size winners: F1,30 ¼ 0.93, P ¼ 0.344; body size losers: F1,30 ¼ 0.37, P ¼ 0.548; sword length losers: F1,30 ¼ 0.41, P ¼ 0.526). Sequential stepwise removal of the least significant variable did not improve the model markedly. This multivariate model was not far removed from the univariate regression of body size-corrected sword length for winners versus log contest duration (N ¼ 31, F1,30 ¼ 7.02, R2 ¼ 0.195, b ¼ %2.01, P ¼ 0.013; Fig. 1). Body size-corrected sword length for losers was not related to log contest duration (F1,30 ¼ 0.99, b ¼ %0.55, P ¼ 0.328). Furthermore, the difference between winners and losers in body size-corrected sword length was not related to contest duration (N ¼ 31, F1,30 ¼ 0.34, R2 ¼ 0.012, b ¼ %0.29, P ¼ 0.566). Similarly, the difference in body size between winners and losers was not related to contest duration (N ¼ 31, F1,30 ¼ 0.54, R2 ¼ 0.018, b ¼ %0.13, P ¼ 0.468). Thus, only body sizecorrected sword length of the winner influenced duration of the contest.

8.5 8 7.5 Log (contest duration)

1984

7 6.5 6 5.5 5 4.5 4 –0.6

–0.4

–0.2 0 0.2 0.4 Body size-corrected sword length

0.6

Figure 1. Bivariate relation between log contest duration and body size-corrected sword length for winners in aggressive contests between male swordtails matched for size.

A specific prediction of the self-assessment model is that contest duration should increase with increasing mean contestant size when rivals are matched for size (Taylor et al. 2001; Taylor & Elwood 2003); however, when this was included in the multivariate regression model, contest duration did not change with mean contestant body size (F1,30 ¼ 1.25, b ¼ 53.13, P ¼ 0.274). The probability that a fight escalated to mutual biting was not influenced by the body size or sword length (corrected for body size) of winners and losers (U ¼ 0.074, N ¼ 31, c23 ¼ 3:00, P ¼ 0.559). Having excluded those fights that were resolved after mutual biting, the probability of further escalation to mouth wrestling was also not related to the body size or sword length (U ¼ 0.078, N ¼ 20, c23 ¼ 2:10, P ¼ 0.718). We repeated the analysis of the probability of escalation to mouth wrestling, for all contests, but again the probability that a contest escalated to mouth wrestling was not affected by size or sword length (U ¼ 0.054, N ¼ 31, c23 ¼ 2:22, P ¼ 0.700). Substituting body size difference or difference in sword length did not improve the models. DISCUSSION Contest outcome was dependent on male size, with winners being greater in length, height and surface area. Such a size advantage is a common feature of animal contests (reviewed in Huntingford & Turner 1987; Andersson 1994; Riechert 1998; Briffa & Sneddon 2007), including those of swordtails (Beaugrand & Zayan 1985; Beaugrand et al. 1991, 1996; Ribowski & Franck 1993; Moretz 2003; Benson & Basolo 2006). Body size may affect outcome by either self-assessment or mutual assessment. To distinguish between these possibilities, we examined whether contest duration increased with mean contestant size, as predicted by the self-assessment model (Taylor & Elwood 2003) but found no significant effect and hence no evidence of self-assessment. The self-assessment model has been found to apply to various species including fiddler crabs, Uca mjoebergi (Morrell et al. 2005), amphipods, Gammarus pulex (Prenter et al. 2006), jumping spiders, Plexippus paykulli (Taylor et al. 2001), tree wetas, Hemideina crassidens (Kelly 2006), dwarf chameleons, Bradypodion pumilum (Stuart-Fox 2006) and fallow deer, Dama dama (Jennings et al. 2004). Because our fish had been size matched, we could not use the regressions between individual body sizes and contest duration to discriminate further between self-assessment and mutual assessment models (Taylor & Elwood 2003). Body size, however, does not provide complete information about fighting ability in swordtails, as smaller contestants can still win contests (Franck & Ribowski 1989) and 25.8% did so in the present study. We explored factors that affect contest outcome in combination by multivariate logistic regression. This indicated that the greater the size difference the more likely the larger fish won. This occurred despite the small variation in size difference and again indicates the importance of body size on outcome. There was, however, no indication of an effect of sword length. This agrees with Benson & Basolo (2006) who noted a body size effect but failed to find an

PRENTER ET AL.: MALEe MALE CONTESTS IN SWORDTAIL FISH

effect of absolute natural sword length on fight outcome. However, it does not agree with their finding that false swords, painted to appear different sizes, influenced outcome. From our data on outcome it seems that natural swords neither enhance nor reduce the chances of victory in these contests. Numerous studies have shown that a key predictor of contest duration is the size difference between the contestants. Regardless of whether mutual or self-assessment occurs, fights are expected to be short when the size difference is large and this has been repeatedly found in a wide variety of species (reviewed in Taylor & Elwood 2003). However, in the present study the low variance in size difference caused by our attempt to size match resulted in this relation between size difference and contest duration being nonsignificant. However, the sword of the winner had a marked effect on the timing of the giving-up decision of the loser because fights were shorter when the winner had a long sword relative to body size (Fig. 1). Having a large sword relative to the body size thus appears to be advantageous in winning quickly. We sought to distinguish between the possibilities that the sword directly affects fighting ability or that it is an honest indicator of condition and hence of fighting ability. The former seems unlikely because it is not used as a weapon and because there are hydrodynamic costs of bearing a long sword (Basolo & Alcaraz 2003; Basolo & Wagner 2004). It seems more likely that the sword acts as an honest signal of condition or fighting ability that allows faster decisions from losers. We presume it is honest because of the hydrodynamic costs shown previously (Basolo & Alcaraz 2003) and a long sword seems to increase predation risk (Basolo & Wagner 2004; see also Rosenthal et al. 2001). However, longer swords are associated with increased ability in fast-start swimming (Royle et al. 2006), so they may be an indicator of, and potentially enhance, fighting ability. Benson & Basolo (2006) noted that males with larger apparent swords were more likely to win even though the actual swords were the same in the two contestants. Thus the hydrodynamic effects must have been the same, and the fighting ability could not have been influenced; the loser must therefore have been responding to a signal of quality. Furthermore, we would have predicted that if the sword confers a physical advantage then winners with large swords would be more likely to escalate the contest and bring about swift resolution by that tactic. However, our data did not support this prediction concerning escalation and we conclude that the sword is a signal of condition rather than being a feature that enhances fighting ability. If the loser gathered information about the condition of the winner via the size of the winner’s sword then this would preclude the self-assessment model of conflict resolution (Taylor & Elwood 2003). Mutual assessment, however, predicts that the sword of the loser and relative sword length should have effects but this was not found. Losers gathered information about the eventual winner but that information seemed not to be used in relation to the loser’s own sword. Furthermore, there was no indication that the winner gathered information about the loser with respect to swords. Thus, while the data preclude

self-assessment, they do not fit the expected pattern of mutual assessment with respect to swords. This appears to be a case of asymmetry in information use that has been seen when animals take different positional roles in fights (Briffa & Elwood 2005). An alternative to the explanation that sword length represents an honest signal of condition might involve the social inhibition of maturation found in related platyfish, Xiphophorus variatus (Borowsky 1973, 1978). The sword may indicate the time since release from inhibition and maturation. If this in turn indicates fighting experience, then males could be assessing the experience of their opponents in contests via assessment of their sword. However, the consequences of this inhibition are unclear and this idea does not sit well with the compensatory growth also known to occur in this species (Royle et al. 2006). It may seem odd that a feature such as sword length can alter tactical decisions such as the time afforded to the contest and yet does not alter the outcome to a significant degree. In this respect our study is similar to those that have shown altered tactics that resulted in only marginal and nonsignificant shifts in probability of victory (e.g. Dugatkin & Ohlsen 1990; Arnott & Elwood 2007; see also Magnhagen 2006 in which information about an opponent available through eavesdropping did not increase the probability of winning a nest site, but did decrease the level of aggression in contests in common gobies, Pomatoschistus microps). In the present study, the main decision about giving up might have been based on body size and reinforced by the eventual loser attending to information gathered about the sword of the winner. A loser that perceives a long sword may thus take this as confirmation of its inferiority whereas a loser that perceives a short sword on a large fish may not be so sure about being inferior and continues to interact for a while. There is a benefit to the winner in having the long sword, however, not because it enhances the probability of victory but because the winner does not have to fight for as long. Presumably, however, a shift in contest duration will have some effect on outcome but that effect may be too small to determine easily through experimentation.

Acknowledgments We are grateful to Ryan Earley, Mark Briffa and Gareth Arnott for discussion and comments on the manuscript. Funding was provided by a grant from the U.K. Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (Research Grant S20306 to R.W.E. and J.P.). We thank Gillian Riddell, Stuart Barr and Gareth Arnott for their assistance in maintaining the fish.

References Andersson, M. 1994. Sexual Selection. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press. Arnott, G. & Elwood, R. W. 2007. Fighting for shells but selling a pup; how private information about resource value changes hermit crab pre-fight displays and escalated fight behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 274, 3011e3017.

1985

1986

ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 75, 6

Basolo, A. L. 1990. Female preference for male sword length in the green swordtail, Xiphophorus helleri (Pisces: Poeciliidae). Animal Behaviour, 40, 332e338. Basolo, A. L. & Alcaraz, G. 2003. The turn of the sword: length increases male swimming costs in swordtails. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 270, 1631e1636. Basolo, A. L. & Trainor, B. C. 2002. The conformation of a female preference for a composite male trait in green swordtails. Animal Behaviour, 63, 469e474. Basolo, A. L. & Wagner, W. E., Jr. 2004. Covariation between predation risk, body size and fin elaboration in the green swordtail. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 83, 87e100. Beaugrand, J. P. 1997. Resolution of agonistic conflicts in dyads of acquainted green swordtails (Xiphophorus helleri): a game with perfect information. Behavioural Processes, 41, 79e96. Beaugrand, J. P. & Beaugrand, M. 1991. Prior residency and the stability of dominance relationships in pairs of green swordtail fish Xiphophorus helleri (Pisces: Poeciliidae). Behavioural Processes, 24, 169e175. Beaugrand, J. P. & Zayan, R. 1985. An experimental model of aggressive dominance in Xiphophorus helleri (Pisces, Poeciliidae). Behavioural Processes, 10, 1e52. Beaugrand, J. P., Goulet, C. & Payette, D. 1991. Outcome of dyadic conflict in male green swordtail fish Xiphophorus helleri: effects of body size and prior dominance. Animal Behaviour, 41, 417e 424. Beaugrand, J. P., Payette, D. & Goulet, C. 1996. Conflict outcome in male green swordtail fish dyads (Xiphophorus helleri): interaction of body size, prior dominance/subordination experience and prior residency. Behaviour, 133, 303e319. Benson, K. E. & Basolo, A. L. 2006. Maleemale competition and the sword in male swordtails, Xiphophorus helleri. Animal Behaviour, 71, 129e134. Borowsky, R. L. 1973. Social control of adult size in males of Xiphophorus variatus. Nature, 245, 332e335. Borowsky, R. L. 1978. Social inhibition of maturation in natural populations of Xiphophorus variatus (Pisces: Poeciliidae). Science, 201, 933e935. Briffa, M. & Elwood, R. W. 2005. Rapid change in energy status in fighting animals: causes and effects of strategic decisions. Animal Behaviour, 70, 119e124. Briffa, M. & Sneddon, L. U. 2007. Physiological constraints on contest behaviour. Functional Ecology, 21, 627e637. Cross, F. R., Jackson, R. R., Pollard, S. D. & Walker, M. W. 2006. Influence of optical cues from conspecific females on escalation decisions during maleemale interactions of jumping spiders. Behavioural Processes, 73, 136e141. Dugatkin, L. A. & Ohlsen, S. R. 1990. Contrasting asymmetries in value expectations and resource holding power: effects on attack behaviour and dominance in the pumpkinseed sunfish, Lepomis gibbosus. Animal Behaviour, 39, 802e804. Earley, R. L. & Dugatkin, L. A. 2002. Eavesdropping on visual cues in green swordtail (Xiphophorus helleri) fights: a case for networking. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 269, 943e 952. Earley, R. L., Druen, M. & Dugatkin, L. A. 2005. Watching fights does not alter a bystander’s response towards na€ıve conspecifics in male green swordtail fish, Xiphophorus helleri. Animal Behaviour, 69, 1139e1145. Francis, R. C. 1983. Experimental effects on aggression behaviour in the paradise fish Mecropodus operculatus. Behaviour, 85, 292e313. Franck, D. & Hendricks, R. 1973. Zur Frage der biologischen Bedeutung der Schwertfortsatzes von Xiphophorus helleri. Ethology, 44, 167e185.

Franck, D. & Ribowski, A. 1987. Influences of prior agonistic experiences on aggression measures in the male swordtail (Xiphophorus helleri). Behaviour, 103, 217e240. Franck, D. & Ribowski, A. 1989. Escalating fights for rank-order position between the male swordtails (Xiphophorus helleri): effects of prior rank-order and information transfer. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 24, 133e143. Franck, D. & Ribowski, A. 1993. Dominance hierarchies of male green swordtails (Xiphophorus helleri) in nature. Journal of Fish Biology, 43, 497e499. Franck, D., Kamroth, B., Taebel-Hellwig, A. & Schartl, M. 1998. Home ranges and satellite tactics of green swordtails (Xiphophorus helleri) in nature. Behavioural Processes, 43, 115e123. Hemens, J. 1966. The ethological significance of the sword in X. helleri (Haeckel). Behaviour, 27, 290e315. Hsu, Y., Earley, R. L. & Wolf, L. I. 2006. Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: mechanisms and contest outcomes. Biological Reviews, 81, 33e74. Huntingford, F. & Turner, A. K. 1987. Animal Conflict. London: Chapman & Hall. Jennings, D. J., Gammell, M. P., Carlin, C. M. & Hayden, T. J. 2004. Effect of body weight, antler length, resource value and experience on fight duration and intensity in fallow deer. Animal Behaviour, 68, 213e221. Kelly, C. D. 2006. Fighting for harems: assessment strategies during maleemale contests in the sexually dimorphic Wellington tree weta. Animal Behaviour, 72, 727e736. Magnhagen, C. 2006. Information about an opponent can decrease aggression in male common gobies competing for nest sites. Animal Behaviour, 71, 879e884. Maynard Smith, J. & Parker, G. H. 1976. The logic of asymmetric contests. Animal Behaviour, 24, 159e175. Moretz, J. A. 2003. Aggression and RHP in the northern swordtail fish, Xiphophorus cortezi: the relationship between size and contest dynamics in maleemale competition. Ethology, 109, 995e1008. Morrell, L. J., Backwell, P. R. Y. & Metcalfe, N. B. 2005. Fighting in fiddler crabs Uca mjoebergi: what determines duration? Animal Behaviour, 70, 653e662. Olsson, M. & Shine, R. 2000. Ownership influences the outcome of maleemale contests in the scincid lizard, Niveoscincus microlepidotus. Behavioral Ecology, 11, 587e590. Parker, G. A. 1974. Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 47, 223e243. Parker, G. A. & Rubenstein, D. I. 1981. Role assessment, reserve strategy, and acquisition of information in asymmetric animal contests. Animal Behaviour, 29, 221e240. Prenter, J., Elwood, R. W. & Taylor, P. W. 2006. Self-assessment by males during energetically costly contests over precopula females in amphipods. Animal Behaviour, 72, 861e868. Ribowski, A. & Franck, D. 1993. Demonstration of strength and concealment of weakness in escalating fights of male swordtails (Xiphophorus helleri). Ethology, 93, 265e274. Riechert, S. E. 1998. Game theory and animal contests. In: Game Theory and Animal Behaviour (Ed. by L. A. Dugatkin & H. K. Reeve), pp. 64e92. New York: Oxford University Press. Rohwer, S. 1975. The social significance of avian winter plumage variability. Evolution, 29, 593e610. Rosenthal, G. G., Flores Martinez, T. Y., Garcia de Leon, F. J. & Ryan, M. J. 2001. Shared preferences by predators and females for male ornaments in swordtails. American Naturalist, 158, 146e154. ¨ m, J. & Metcalfe, N. B. 2005. A poor start in Royle, N. J., Lindstro life negatively affects dominance status in adulthood independent of body size in green swordtails Xiphophorus helleri. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 272, 1917e1922.

PRENTER ET AL.: MALEe MALE CONTESTS IN SWORDTAIL FISH

¨ m, J. 2006. Sexual seRoyle, N. J., Metcalfe, N. B. & Lindstro lection, growth compensation and fast-start swimming in green swordtails, Xiphophorus helleri. Functional Ecology, 20, 662e669. Searcy, W. A. & Nowicki, S. 2005. The Evolution of Animal Behaviour. Reliability and Deception in Signalling Systems. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press. Stocker, A. M. & Huber, R. 2001. Fighting strategies in crayfish Orconectes rusticus (Decapoda, Cambaridae) differ with hunger state and the presence of food cues. Ethology, 107, 727e736.

Stuart-Fox, D. 2006. Testing game theory models: fighting ability and decision rules in chameleon contests. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 272, 1555e1561. Taylor, P. W. & Elwood, R. W. 2003. The mis-measure of animal contests. Animal Behaviour, 65, 1195e1202. Taylor, P. W. & Jackson, R. R. 2003. Interacting effects of size and prior injury in jumping spider conflicts. Animal Behaviour, 65, 787e794. Taylor, P. W., Hasson, O. & Clark, D. L. 2001. Initiation and resolution of jumping spider contests: roles for size, proximity and early detection of rivals. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50, 403e413.

1987

Large body size for winning and large swords for ...

period of 64 h without social interaction to mitigate the behavioural effects of prior ..... Social control of adult size in males of Xipho- phorus variatus. Nature, 245 ...

191KB Sizes 2 Downloads 275 Views

Recommend Documents

pdf to word converter for large size files
File: Pdf to word converter for large size. files. Download now. Click here if your download doesn't start automatically. Page 1 of 1. pdf to word converter for large size files. pdf to word converter for large size files. Open. Extract. Open with. S

Higher albedos and size distribution of large ...
Solar System, Kuiper belt, transneptunian belt, albedos, size distribution .... Formation of fresh ice deposits would be feasible only in big bodies with enough ...

Large Corpus Experiments for Broadcast News Recognition
Decoding. The BN-STT (Broadcast News Speech To Text) system pro- ceeds in two stages. The first-pass decoding uses gender- dependent models according ...

PENALTY FUNCTION MAXIMIZATION FOR LARGE ...
on multiple LVCSR tasks show a good correlation between the ob- ..... tion procedure described in [11] (with a lattice n-best degree of 8). Next, we accumulate ...

Are Clouds Ready for Large Distributed Applications?
Page 1 ... Security and Network Components ... Transformation of existing enterprise service deployment into a cloud-based deployment. – Discovery of ...

Automatic Reconfiguration for Large-Scale Reliable Storage ...
Automatic Reconfiguration for Large-Scale Reliable Storage Systems.pdf. Automatic Reconfiguration for Large-Scale Reliable Storage Systems.pdf. Open.

Networking Best Practices for Large Deployments
Nov 16, 2016 - rights relating to the Google services are and shall remain the exclusive ..... 1. Network Evaluation: This section contains information on ...

Large Place value for wall.pdf
Large Place value for wall.pdf. Large Place value for wall.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu. Displaying Large Place value for wall.pdf. Page 1 of ...

SPEAKER SELECTION TRAINING FOR LARGE ...
adaptation methods such as MAP and MLLR [1] do not work well. ... may be a good index to select the reference speakers from many ... phone (or phone class).

Support-Theoretic Subgraph Preconditioners for Large ... - CiteSeerX
significantly improve the efficiency of the state-of-the-art solver. I. INTRODUCTION ... 1: Illustration of the proposed algorithm with a simple grid graph. (a) The ...

Generating Precise Dependencies for Large Software
Abstract—Intra- and inter-module dependencies can be a significant source of technical debt in the long-term software development, especially for large ...

PENALTY FUNCTION MAXIMIZATION FOR LARGE ...
state-level Hamming distance versus a negative phone-level ac- curacy. Indeed ... The acoustic features for the English system are 40-dimensional vectors obtained via an .... [3] F. Sha and L. Saul, “Comparison of large margin training to other ...

Support-Theoretic Subgraph Preconditioners for Large ... - CiteSeerX
develop an algorithm to find good subgraph preconditioners and apply them ... metric based on support theory to measure the quality of a spanning tree ...... SDD linear systems,” in Symp. on Foundations of Computer Science. (FOCS), 2011.

Cheap 2017 New Super Large Size Optional Mouse Pad Natural ...
... Mouse Pa ... k Gaming Mousepad Mats for csgo dota 2 LOL CF.pdf. Cheap 2017 New Super Large Size Optional Mouse Pad ... sk Gaming Mousepad Mats for ...

Algorithms for Linear and Nonlinear Approximation of Large Data
become more pertinent in light of the large amounts of data that we ...... Along with the development of richer representation structures, recently there has.

Enhanced NURBS modeling and visualization for large ...
Conference on Geologic Remote Sensing, Pasadena, CA, pp. 221–231. Piegl, L., Tiller, W., 1997. The NURBS Book, second ed. Springer, New York, NY 650pp.

Compact Representation for Large-Scale Clustering and Similarity ...
in high-dimensional feature space for large-scale image datasets. In this paper, ... the clustering method to avoid high-dimension indexing. [5] also states that the.

Functional Programming for Dynamic and Large Data ...
Large Data with Self-Adjusting Computation. Yan Chen. Max Planck ...... they can be quite complex and difficult to design, analyze, and implement even for ...

Low Noise Amplifiers for Small and Large Area ... - Linear Technology
Introduction. Photodiodes can be broken into two categories: large area photodiodes with their attendant high capacitance. (30pF to 3000pF) and smaller area ...

efficient and effective plagiarism detection for large code ... - CiteSeerX
1 School of Computer Science and Information Technology,. RMIT University ... our approach is highly scalable while maintaining similar levels of effectiveness to that of JPlag. .... Our experiments with an online text-based plagiarism detection ...

practical valves and pumps for large-scale integration ...
Micro Total Analysis Systems 2002, Nov. ... nL) dead volumes to the fluidic system. ... metering specific nanoliter- or microliter-scale volumes of fluids may be ...