524

Commentaries

DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00632.x

COMMENTARIES

Blackwell Publishing Ltd

How does the mirror neuron system change during development? James M. Kilner1 and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore2 1. Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College London, UK 2. Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University College London, UK

This is a commentary on Lepage and Théoret (2007).

It has been suggested that the mirror neuron system (MNS) enables the understanding of others’ intentions by simply observing their actions. As Lepage and Théoret’s paper highlights, while the MNS has been extensively studied in adult humans and in monkeys, very little is known about its development. What properties of the MNS change during development, or even when in development a functioning MNS is present, is unknown. Lepage and Théoret review the literature and, on this basis, suggest that the MNS is present by infancy. Here, we argue that, even if this is the case, this does not preclude further developmental change. An understanding of how the MNS develops would shed light on its mechanisms and functions. The notion that actions are intrinsically linked to perception was proposed by William James, who suggested that ‘every mental representation of a movement awakens to some degree the actual movement which is its object’ (James, 1890, p. 293). The implication is that observing, imagining, preparing, or in any way representing an action excites the motor program used to execute that same action (Jeannerod, 1994; Prinz, 1997). Interest in this idea has grown recently, in part due to the neurophysiological discovery of mirror neurons and, in turn, the mirror neuron system (MNS). Mirror neurons in monkey premotor area F5 and inferior parietal lobule (PF) discharge not only during action execution but also during action observation, which has led many to suggest that these neurons are the neural substrate for action understanding. As Lepage and Théoret’s paper highlights, although the MNS has been extensively studied in both adult humans and monkeys, very little is known about the development of the MNS. What properties of the MNS

change during development, or even when in development a functioning MNS is present, is unknown. Lepage and Théoret’s paper addresses this latter issue and they suggest that there are sufficient data to conclude that the MNS is present in infancy. However, this does not preclude further developmental change to the MNS, and an understanding of this would shed light on the mechanisms and functions of the MNS. A recent theoretical account of the MNS predicts that it is the strength of the connections between areas active during action observation that are modulated during development. This theoretical account proposes that the role of the MNS in reading or recognizing the goals of observed actions is best understood within a predictive coding framework (Kilner, Friston & Frith, 2007). The predictive coding framework is a form of recognition model that enables perceptual inference when the mapping between causes and inputs is ill-posed. For example, if while walking along the street someone suddenly waves their arm, are they hailing a taxi or swatting a wasp? In other words, this form of recognition model can infer the goal of an observed action even when the same observed kinematics can be caused by more than one goal or intention. In this type of model, recognition is the inverse of generating sensory data from their causes. Generative models can be framed in terms of a deterministic non-linear generative function: u = G (v, θ) In the case of action execution/observation, u is the input (the visual signal corresponding to the sight of the executed action) and v is a vector of underlying causes

Address for correspondence: James M. Kilner, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College London, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK; e-mail: [email protected] © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA.

Commentaries

(the long-term intentions or goals of the action). G(v, θ) is a function that generates inputs from the causes given some parameters of the model, θ. The inverse of the generative model is recognition, where the goal is to infer the cause, v, given the data, u. This is called perceptual inference. Importantly, in perceptual inference the model parameters, θ, are fixed quantities that have to be learned. The estimation of these parameters of the generative model corresponds to perceptual learning (see Friston, 2002, 2003, for a detailed account of perceptual learning and inference). In neural models, these parameters, θ, are the between-area connection strengths (Friston, 2002, 2003). In other words, the ability of the MNS to infer the cause of any observed action correctly is dependent upon the between-area connection strengths, which have to be learned. There are two ways in which these connection strengths can be modulated: either new connections can be made de novo or the strength of existing connections can be modulated. In the following section we will suggest that during childhood the connection strengths are modulated by the generation of new connections whereas later in development (adolescence) the connectivity is modulated by changes in the connection strengths of pre-existing connections. In their article, Lepage and Théoret focus on the existence of an MNS in infants. However, there are at least two reasons to suppose that properties of the MNS might continue to develop well beyond infancy. First, some of the brain areas that constitute the human MNS, in particular the inferior parietal cortex, continue to develop well beyond early childhood. Recent MRI studies have shown that the parietal cortex develops throughout adolescence and beyond (e.g. Giedd, Blumenthal, Jeffries, Castellanos, Liu, Zijdenbos, Paus, Evans & Rapoport, 1999; see Blakemore & Choudhury, 2006, for review). Grey matter in the parietal cortex follows an inverted U-shaped pattern of development, with grey matter volume increasing during childhood, peaking at age 10.2 (on average) for girls and 11.8 for boys, and then decreasing during adolescence. This pattern of grey matter development has been attributed to the processes of synaptogenesis during childhood followed by synaptic pruning during adolescence (cf. Huttenlocher, 1979). According to the theoretical account outlined earlier, the period of synaptogenesis during childhood would correspond to making connections de novo, whereas the synaptic pruning that occurs during adolescence would correspond to a modulation of the strength of existing connections. Such development might impact on the functions of the developing brain areas – including, for the parietal cortex, MNS functions. The second reason to speculate that properties of the MNS might continue to develop after childhood has a behavioural basis. Adolescence is a time when peer © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

525

relations are of critical importance. It is perhaps more important for adolescents than for adults (or children) to mirror their peers’ behaviour; anecdotally, adolescents tend to copy the likes and dislikes of their peers. In this case, it is possible that MNS activity peaks during adolescence. Alternatively, the MNS might not be fully mature by adolescence, and different strategies might be used by adolescents and adults to make sense of others’ actions. Even if the basic capabilities of the MNS do not change during adolescence, it is possible that its interaction with other brain areas (or its responsivity to modulatory influences) might develop. Several neuroimaging studies of the MNS have included a group of typically developing children or adolescents (e.g. Ohnishi, Moriguchi, Matsuda, Mori, Hirakata, Imabayashi, Hirao, Nemoto, Kaga, Inagaki, Yamada & Uno, 2004; Dapretto, Davies, Pfeifer, Scott, Sigman, Bookheimer & Iacoboni, 2006; Lepage & Théoret, 2006; Williams, Waiter, Gilchrist, Perrett, Murray & Whiten, 2006). However, none has directly compared MNS function in children, adolescents and adults. The notion that the MNS might change during childhood and adolescence is currently speculative, and future research is needed to test these predictions.

Acknowledgements JMK is funded by the Wellcome Trust and SJB is funded by the Royal Society. We are grateful to C. Frith, T. Chaminade, C. Sebastian, M. Garrido and S. Burnett for commenting on earlier drafts of this commentary.

References Blakemore, S.J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47 (3–4), 296–312. Dapretto, M., Davies, M.S., Pfeifer, J.H., Scott, A.A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S.Y., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9 (1), 28–30. Friston, K.J. (2002). Functional integration and inference in the brain. Progress in Neurobiology, 68, 113–143. Friston, K.J. (2003). Learning and inference in the brain. Neural Networks, 16, 1325–1352. Giedd, J.N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N.O., Castellanos, F.X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., Paus, T., Evans, A.C., & Rapoport, J.L. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2 (10), 861–863. Huttenlocher, P.R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex – developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163 (2), 195–205.

526

Commentaries

James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt. Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain – neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 187–202. Kilner, J.M., Friston, K.J., & Frith, C.D. (2007). The mirror-neuron system: a Bayesian perspective. NeuroReport, 18 (6), 619–623. Lepage, J.-F., & Théoret, H. (2006). EEG evidence for the presence of an action observation– execution matching system in children. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23 (9), 2505 –2510. Lepage, J.-F., & Théoret, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system: grasping others’ actions from birth? Developmental Science, DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00631.x

Ohnishi, T., Moriguchi, Y., Matsuda, H., Mori, T., Hirakata, M., Imabayashi, E., Hirao, K., Nemoto, K., Kaga, M., Inagaki, M., Yamada, M., & Uno, A. (2004). The neural network for the mirror system and mentalizing in normally developed children: an fMRI study. NeuroReport, 15 (9), 1483–1487. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154. Williams, J.H., Waiter, G.D., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D.I., Murray, A.D., & Whiten, A. (2006). Neural mechanisms of imitation and ‘mirror neuron’ functioning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 44 (4), 610–621.

DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00633.x

Is there evidence of a mirror system from birth? Bennett I. Bertenthal1,2 and Matthew R. Longo2,3 1. College of Arts and Sciences, Indiana University, USA 2. Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, USA 3. Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, University College London, UK

This is a commentary on Lepage and Théoret (2007). Recent neurophysiological, neuropsychological, and behavioral evidence suggests that one of the mechanisms responsible for understanding others’ actions is a shared representation mediated by a human homologue of the monkey’s mirror neuron system (e.g. Bertenthal, Longo & Kosobud, 2006; Decety & Sommerville, 2004; Frith & Frith, 2006; Grèzes, Frith & Passingham, 2004). The neural circuit responsible for this shared representation could be present from birth or could develop as a function of learning and experience. In the target article by Lepage and Théoret, the authors speculate that a dedicated neural system is present from birth based primarily on evidence of neonatal imitation, although they acknowledge that neurophysiological evidence is required to definitively establish the presence of a mirror neuron system. We concur with this conclusion, but believe that the evidence for neonatal imitation and how it relates to the development of covert imitation needs to be examined in more detail. In the remainder of this commentary we will discuss how changes in neonatal imitation over real and developmental time support the hypothesis of a functional mirror system from birth. We will also discuss

additional behavioral evidence supporting the presence of this system in somewhat older infants, which suggests that the transition from overt to covert imitation following action observation emerges relatively early in development. By definition, the mirror system involves the observation of an action directly mapping onto the motor representation of that action. If a specific motor representation is not yet present or is poorly developed, then the observed action will be less likely to stimulate the execution of a comparable response (Calvo-Merino, Glaser, Grèzes, Passingham & Haggard, 2005; Longo, Kosobud & Bertenthal, 2007). This necessity for an internal description or representation of a motor response helps to explain why the imitation of orofacial gestures are such good candidates for imitation via a mirror system. It is well established that fetuses perform mouth opening and closing and tongue protrusion while in utero (Prechtl, 1986). Thus, these gestures are already part of the neonate’s behavioral repertoire at birth, suggesting that through practice and exercise a neural network for executing these behaviors has been established. Moreover, the neuroanatomical evidence shows that the corticobulbar tract

Address for correspondence: Bennett I. Bertenthal, Indiana University, College of Arts and Sciences, Kirkwood Hall, 104, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA; e-mail: [email protected] © 2007 The Authors. Journal compilation © 2007 Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

How does the mirror neuron system change during development?

studied in adult humans and in monkeys, very little is known about its .... social cognition. Journal of ... network for the mirror system and mentalizing in normally.

66KB Sizes 0 Downloads 272 Views

Recommend Documents

Contagious yawning: The mirror neuron system may be ...
3400 Civic Centre Boulevard,. Philadelphia, PA 19104,. United States. Tel. ... Indeed, recent data show that cell cycle regulator cdc2 is required for asymmetric.

The mirror-neuron system: a Bayesian perspective - UCSD Cognitive ...
view and the results of recent studies suggest that the inferior parietal lobule area may be ... 2005; 360:815–836. 16. Wolpert DM, Doya K, Kawato M. A unifying ...

Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system - Springer Link
Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system. James M. Kilner Æ Karl J. Friston Æ Chris D. Frith. Received: 21 February 2007 / Revised: 19 March ...

How Does Multinational Production Change ...
factors that trigger FDI entry and expand foreign sales over the business cycle. ...... The increase of the foreign wages reduces profit opportunities for home.

EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism ...
Mar 9, 2005 - and empathy [12], all of which are known to be impaired in individuals with autism .... imitation is common when humans and other primates interact ... proposed that theory of mind is the core deficit in autism, which leads to ..... scr

How Does The Political System Influence The ...
How Does The Political System Influence The. Economic Growth In ... hence by means of our study we could observe the impact that the political system .... concluded that the results for Romania confirm and comply with the trend given by the ...

Motor control may support mirror neuron research with new ...
Mar 3, 2015 - regards the fundamental definition of what exactly synergies might be. ..... movements not complying with the synergistic organization of the ...

does not compute - Change the Equation
sobering new data on millennials' skills and attitudes. The findings offer clear ... According to recent research, for example, 80 percent of middle-skill jobs, which.

does not compute - Change the Equation
media,1 yet CTEq's analysis finds that 58 percent have low skills in solving ... of computer applications. For example ... 70 percent of the 35–64-year-old set have low skills—but that's cold ... generally require less than a bachelor's degree, r

Heritage Trends in the Mirror of Social Change ... -
encouraging way of traditional exchange of opinion in conference reports, ... Str. 7, Riga, LV-1050,LATVIA; Phones: +371-6779 6040 or +371-2707 1098; e-mail:.

Heritage Trends in the Mirror of Social Change ... -
encouraging way of traditional exchange of opinion in conference reports, as well as thematically ... and activate the criteria and principles of the Charter,.

Does Reading During the Summer Build Reading Skills? Evidence ...
from, we take advantage of the multi-site nature of the experimental design – there ... regardless of the child's focus during the activity, or does reading only build.

pdf-1831\climatic-change-in-the-eastern-mediterranean-area-during ...
Page 1 of 1. ALAT PERAGA MENARA HANOI, POLA SUDUT, DAN BLOK LOGIKA. Dosen Pembimbing : Dr. Warli. M.pd. Disusun oleh : Abi Fusawat Sarji Rindi Dwi Kurniawati. Page 1 of 1. pdf-1831\climatic-change-in-the-eastern-mediterranean-area-during-the-past-240

Does monetary policy lose effectiveness during a credit ...
Sep 8, 2009 - We get similar results using total loans data, ... 1 We define each period from the onset of slowdown to the onset of recovery in loan growth.

Social cognitive development during adolescence - Oxford Journals
However, the development of social cognition during adolescence and its neural underpinnings remains .... context of the Social Information Processing Network.

Development of action representation during ...
figure 8s with a pencil, within the parallel pair of lines, as quickly and as accurately as they could. There were three size conditions of the figure 8: small, medium.

Does the Human Motor System Simulate Pinocchio's ...
sent in the individual go/no-go task because only one response code was formed and no conflict between spatial codes occurred. Address correspondence to Chia-Chin Tsai, Institute of Neurosci- ence, National Yang Ming University, 155, Sec 2, Li Long S

Does the Human Motor System Simulate Pinocchio's ... - SAGE Journals
1Institute of Neuroscience, National Yang Ming University, Taipei, Taiwan; 2Laboratories for Cognitive Neuroscience,. National Yang Ming University, Taipei, ...

Does Content-Focused Teacher Professional Development Work?
Three recent random assignment studies from the Institute of Education ...... 1 The large investment in PD is one policy response to continuing concerns about ...

aldon change management system pdf
File: Aldon change management system. pdf. Download now. Click here if your download doesn't start automatically. Page 1 of 1. aldon change management system pdf. aldon change management system pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu. Displ

Estimating System State During Human Walking With a ...
Clinically prescribed AFO systems assist impaired individuals by providing support for .... ESTIMATING SYSTEM STATE DURING HUMAN WALKING WITH A POWERED ANKLE-FOOT ORTHOSIS. 837 the same rate from two ..... from 10 to 7.1 and the event detection error

how does pennsylvania's tax burden compare? - Commonwealth ...
Aug 25, 2016 - The 2nd highest corporate income tax rate in the U.S (9.9%). Combined with the ... 8th highest state and local cell phone tax rate (14.14%).