Electronic Supplementary Material S1

Food sharing in vampire bats: reciprocal help predicts donations more than relatedness or harassment Gerald G. Carter* and Gerald S. Wilkinson Department of Biology, University of Maryland, College Park, USA, 20742 *[email protected] Supplement to Methods We carried out our study in accordance with the Animal Behavior Society Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research [1], and the Organization for Bat Conservation (Bloomfield Hills, MI, USA).

Microsatellite analysis We used the web program PRIMER 3 (http://frodo.wi.mit.edu/) to design primers for 12 Desmodus rotundus DNA sequences with repeats [2] in GenBank, and added 1 additional published microsatellite marker, Tsil3Ca2 [3]. For these 13 microsatellite loci [4], we compared the frequency, and observed and expected heterozygosities from the 25 bats genotyped in our population with 16 free-ranging adult common vampire bats netted in Trinidad, West Indies. We failed to find a significant difference in heterozygosities between observed and expected values (paired t=1.85, n=13, p>0.05) or between the two populations (paired t=1.2, n=13, p>0.05). We used the programs ML-RELATE [5] and MICROCHECKER [6] to check and account for potential scoring errors and null alleles. For 3 loci, we used ML-RELATE to calculate maximum likelihood estimates of the frequency of null alleles for all calculations estimating pairwise relatedness [7]. Sequential analyses To determine if individual food donations were exchanged in a reciprocal manner over time, we examined the sequence of sharing events across trials. For this analysis we only analysed trials where donors were previously subjects. We also only included mouth-licking bouts longer than 15 s to remove instances of begging. We then tested (1) if the donation size from bat B to bat A in a trial was predicted by the size of the most recent previous donation from A to B, and (2) if the percentage of B’s contribution to the total food received by A in a trial was predicted by the percentage of A’s contribution to the total food received by B in the most recent previous donation from A to B. Consistency analysis To test for evidence of symmetrical and consistent dyadic relationships over time, we examined the amount (food donated/chances to give) and the presence or absence of food sharing for dyads that had multiple chances to donate. We used Mantel and randomization tests to assess similarity of (1) presence and amounts of food sharing in subsequent fasting rounds, (2) presence and amounts of food sharing six months apart, (3) allogrooming given and received within dyads, and (4) the food-sharing matrix with its transpose, using bats that both served as subjects and were available as donors in every round (67% of the total possible food sharing dyads). Supplement to Results

Pattern of food sharing We induced food sharing on 48 out of 52 days over a period of 780 days. Overall patterns of food sharing are shown in Figure S2. Female adults and juveniles (4-8 months old) were always fed by at least one other bat, while 4 of 9 adult males were never fed. After controlling for chances to give and receive, adult females donated 78% and received 57% of the total amount of food donated, while juveniles provided and received an additional 13% and 15% respectively. We observed no food sharing between adult males. However, we did observe two unrelated adult males feeding a male juvenile, and sharing between two unrelated 8 month-old males. Sixty-three of the 98 dyads that shared food had relatedness estimates less than 0.05 (Figure S2). This percentage (64%) is close to the number expected if partners were chosen at random with respect to relatedness because 208 of 312 possible food-sharing dyads (67%) were related by less than 0.05. Recipients were fed by an average of 3.9 donors in a trial (range=1-7). The median food donated

from a donor to recipient in a trial was 191 s (N=204 donations, mean=339 s, range=5-3315 s). When scaling by chances to give, female donors gave food to recipients for an average of 256 s (S.E.=45), and male donors gave for an average of 164 s (S.E.=89). One feeding donation lasting 3315 s from an adult female to a highly related two-year old male was an extreme outlier (Figure S1). The total amount of food received from all donors during the 2 h period was typically about 5% of an adult recipient’s mass, which restored ~20% of mass lost during 24 h of fasting. The donation during a trial consisted of several mouth-licking bouts, and the median length of a mouth-licking bout was 40 s (mean=79.9, S.D.=131.6 s).

Predictors of food sharing across dyads Food received (p<0.0002), donor sex (p<0.0002), and allogrooming received (p=0.0056), but not relatedness (p=0.2896), predicted the presence of food sharing among the 312 dyads where food sharing could have occurred (Figure S4, S5). However, within the subset of 98 dyads that shared food, food donation size was predicted by relatedness (R2=0.063, p=0.0032) and food received (R2=0.049, p=0.0324) in separate univariate analyses. We found no significant predictors when these factors were considered simultaneously. The effect of relatedness was driven largely by extended maternal care. Mothers feeding their pups or putative subadult offspring (ages 4-31 months) constitute four of the largest donations (Figure S6, three largest outliers in Figure S1). If these dyads are removed, relatedness is no longer a significant predictor (p=0.4). Supplementary Figures

Figure S1. Distribution of mean mouth-licking times before and after log transformation. Mouthlicking time was used to estimate food donated to the subject. Box shows the distance of the interquartile range, the whiskers extend that range 1.5 times, and the line within the box is the median.

 

 

Figure S2. Food donated values shown as a sociomatrix. Columns are donors and rows are fasted subjects. Blank squares have zero values and black squares are dyads that did not have an opportunity to donate or receive food. Dyads are coloured according to estimates of pairwise relatedness (r): white for r estimates 0—0.05, light green for r estimates between 0.05—0.25, and blue for r estimates >0.25. Numbers are mean seconds of food sharing per trial. Unit for age is months.

Figure S3. Box plot showing food donated by donor sex. Box shows the distance of the interquartile range, the whiskers extend that range 1.5 times, and the line within the box is the median.

Figure S4. Logistic regression plot. The effect of log food received and relatedness on the presence of food sharing is shown.

Figure S5. Frequency histograms showing proportion of dyads that shared food across values of food received and relatedness. Grey bars show dyads that could have shared food in both directions and black bars show dyads that did share food. Length of bars above horizontal line is reduced by 95% to fit graph (length is equally reduced above line for both histograms).

Figure S6. Relationship between relatedness and food donated. Values for mothers feeding juvenile or putative adult offspring are shown as open circles.

 

References 1

2012 Animal Behavior Society guidelines for the use of animals in research. Anim Behav 83, 301–309. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.031)

2

Piaggio, A., Johnston, J. & Perkins, S. 2008 Development of polymorphic microsatellite loci for the common vampire bat, Desmodus rotundus (Chiroptera, Phyllostomidae). Mol Ecol Resources 8, 440-442.

3

Dechmann, D., Garbely, E., Kerth, G. & Garner, T. 2002 Highly polymorphic microsatellites for the study of the round-eared bat, Tonatia silvicola (d’Orbigny). Conserv Genet 3, 455–458.

4

Carter, G. G. & Wilkinson, G. S. 2012 Data from: Food sharing in vampire bats: reciprocal help predicts donations more than relatedness or harassment. Dryad Digital Repository. (doi:10.5061/dryad.tg7b1)

5

Kalinowski, S. T., Wagner, A. P. & Taper, M. L. 2006 ML-RELATE: a computer program for maximum likelihood estimation of relatedness and relationship. Mol Ecol Notes 6, 576–579. (doi:10.1111/j.1471-8286.2006.01256.x)

6

Van Oosterhout, C., Hutchinson, W. F., Wills, D. P. M. & Shipley, P. 2004 Microchecker: software for identifying and correcting genotyping errors in microsatellite data. Mol Ecol Notes 4, 535–538. (doi:10.1111/j.1471-8286.2004.00684.x)

7

Kalinowski, S. T. & Taper, M. L. 2006 Maximum likelihood estimation of the frequency of null alleles at microsatellite loci. Conserv Genet 7, 991–995. (doi:10.1007/0592006-9134-9)

Electronic Supplementary Material S1 Food sharing in ...

Food sharing in vampire bats: reciprocal help predicts donations more than .... Kalinowski, S. T., Wagner, A. P. & Taper, M. L. 2006 ML-RELATE: a computer ...

1MB Sizes 0 Downloads 232 Views

Recommend Documents

Electronic supplementary material
Jun 22, 2009 - ... of two Hill functions (a-b) with sufficiently distant midpoints is equivalent to the Hill function with the largest midpoint (c). Namely: Hill(x, K 1) · Hill(x, K 2) ≈ Hill(x, K 2) where K 1

Electronic Supplementary Material- Optimizing ...
3Data Science Laboratory, Institute for Scientific Interchange (ISI), Turin, Italy. 4Istituto ..... The entropy function introduced in Equation 1 of the main text evaluates the level of frag- ..... In the case of a deterministic spreading, we recover

Supplementary Material
Jan Heufer ∗. *TU Dortmund University, Department of Economics and Social Science, 44221 Dortmund,. Germany. .... 3 More Details on the Data Analysis.

Supplementary Material
gaze fixation point is detected by an eye tracker. ∗indicates equal contribution. (a) Random ... 1) confirms quanti- tatively our intuition, that the location of the hands are in- deed important for learning about hand-object interactions. Based on

Supplementary Material - Arkivoc
General Papers. ARKIVOC 2015 (vii) S1-S13. Page S3. ©ARKAT-USA, Inc. δ /ppm. 1. H Assignment. 8.60 (brs, 1H). 5.35 (t, J = 7.3 Hz, 1H). 5.08 (t, J = 6.1 Hz, ...

Supplementary Material
By the definition of conjunctive pattern, since S does not satisfy pc, then S ... Then there exists at least one instance of p o in S: {I1,I2,...,Im},. I1 ∈ I(p c. 1 ,S),...,Im ∈ I(p c m,S), such that ∀t1 ∈. I1,..., ∀tm ∈ Im,t1 < t2 < ...

Supplementary Material
and Business Cycles Conference, the Bank of Canada 2014 Fellowship ..... CGH exhibited an upward trend in the frequency of sales that could explain why they find ...... 12Fox and Sayed (2016) use the IRI scanner dataset to document the ...

Supplementary Material
The data are provided a for 10 subsectors, which I aggregate up to three sectors as follows. ... CAN Canada ...... Structural change in an open economy. Journal ...

Supplementary Material
SVK Slovakia. CYP Cyprus. ITA. Italy. SVN Slovenia. CZE Czech Republic JPN Japan. SWE Sweden. DEU Germany. KOR South Korea. TUR Turkey. DNK Denmark. LTU Lithuania. TWN Taiwan. ESP Spain. LUX Luxembourg. USA United States. EST Estonia. LVA Latvia. RoW

Efficient Repeated Implementation: Supplementary Material
strategy bi except that at (ht,θt) it reports z + 1. Note from the definition of mechanism g∗ and the transition rules of R∗ that such a deviation at (ht,θt) does not ...

Supplementary Material for
Aug 3, 2016 - alternatives are “autocracy and risk-sharing” and “democracy and investment.” In other words, the .... whether seizing power. A coup is .... Our main sources are Galasso (1976), Stearns (2001), and Ortu (2005). References.

Supplementary Online Material
Branstetter, M.G. (2009) The ant genus Stenamma Westwood (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) redefined, with a description of a new genus Propodilobus. Zootaxa,.

Supplementary Material for
Fujitsu Research & Development Center Co., Ltd, Beijing, China. {wangzhengxiang,rjliu}@cn.fujitsu.com. Abstract. This supplementary material provides the ...

Supplementary Material - HEC Montréal
... the ONS website: http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/datasets-and-tables/index.html. .... sitivity of sales to the unemployment rate for independent stores and chains.

Supplementary Material for
Sep 25, 2015 - Using archived and field-collected worker bumble bees, we tested for temporal changes in the tongue ... (Niwot Ridge Long Term Ecological Research Site, 40°3.567'N, 105°37.000'W), and by. P. A. Byron (17) ... these transects did not

Supplementary Material for ``Observations on ...
Nov 19, 2017 - C.2 Steady State in a Perturbed Environment. In this subsection we formally adapt the definitions of a consistent signal profile and of a steady state to perturbed environments. Let f((1−ϵ)·σ+ϵ·λ) : OS → OS be the mapping bet

Supplementary Material Methods Tables
αF was added (25nM final) to exponentially growing MATa bar1Δ cells in YPD (OD600 ≈ 0.3) and the culture incubated for two hours at. 30oC. >95% G1 arrest, as indicated by cells with small buds (schmoo formation), was confirmed by microscopy. Cell

Supplementary Material: Adaptive Consensus ADMM ...
inequalities together. We conclude. (Bvk+1 − Bvk)T (λk+1 − λk) ≥ 0. (S3). 1.2. Proof of ..... matrix Di ∈ Rni×d with ni samples and d features using a standard ...

Price Selection – Supplementary Material
†Email: Departamento de Economia, Pontifıcia Universidade Católica do Rio de .... and-tables/index.html, the Statistics Canada's Consumer Price Research ...

Price Selection – Supplementary Material
trade and search frictions in the goods market. An equilibrium pins down a unique relative price distribution G(p−1 | S−1) but does not pin down price changes. This distribution is invariant to monetary shocks. Hence there is full monetary neutra

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL FOR “WEAK MONOTONICITY ...
This representation is convenient for domains with complete orders. 1 .... check dominant-strategy implementability of many classical social choice rules. In.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL FOR “WEAK MONOTONICITY ...
This representation is convenient for domains with complete orders. 1 ... v = (0,v2,0), v2 > 0, would want to deviate and misreport their type so as to get 3.

The Effect of Attractiveness on Food Sharing Preferences in Human ...
Michael Gumert, Division of Psychology, School of Humanities and Social Sciences, ... Keywords: food sharing, mating market, facial attractiveness, dating, mate ...

The Effect of Attractiveness on Food Sharing Preferences in Human ...
sex interest could have been used elsewhere; perhaps on another date, ... bank account. Higher ..... Why do good hunters have higher reproductive success?