1 2 3

Applied Animal Behaviour Science

4 5

Final revision – NOT EDITED by the journal

Doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.02.005

6

Effects of cage-cleaning frequency on laboratory rat

7

reproduction, cannibalism, and welfare

8

Charlotte C. Burn1* and Georgia J. Mason2

9 10

1

Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3PS,

11

UK and 2Department of Animal & Poultry Science, University of Guelph, Ontario,

12

N1G 2W1, Canada

13 14 15 16 17 18

Running title: Cage-cleaning frequency effects on breeding rats

19 20 21 22 23

*School of Clinical Veterinary Science, University of Bristol, Langford House,

24

Bristol BS40 5DU, UK

1

25

Abstract

26

Regular cage-cleaning is important for health, but for breeding rats it disrupts the nest

27

and removes olfactory signals important for parental care. To investigate how

28

different cage-cleaning frequencies affect breeding rats' health and welfare, we

29

monitored reproductive output, pup mortality, pup sex-ratios, parental

30

chromodacryorrhoea and in-cage ammonia levels for rats in a commercial breeding

31

facility. Cages were cleaned twice-weekly, once-weekly or every two weeks (18

32

cages/group), replicated in two buildings, for the entire 36-week reproductive period.

33

Frequent cage-cleaning had no clear benefits or major negative effects, showing no

34

significant reductions in ammonia levels, or affects on health or overall pup mortality.

35

However, frequent cage-cleaning slightly but significantly increased cannibalistic

36

behaviour. This was because: (i) vulnerable 0-2 day old pups were more likely to be

37

exposed to a cage-cleaning event in the more frequent cage-cleaning regimes,

38

physically disturbing them, and disrupting the nest and scent marks; and (ii) in the

39

twice-weekly and weekly-cleaned groups, pups under 2 days old at their first cage-

40

cleaning were more likely to be cannibalised. Possible mechanisms behind these

41

effects are discussed, including that cleaning might induce premature births, or stress

42

the parents through noise or olfactory and physical disturbance. Finally, the cage-

43

cleaning frequency producing most pups differed between the two buildings – an

44

interactive effect corroborating previous findings that same-strain rodents’ phenotypes

45

can differ with environment. Overall, we suggest that for breeding rats, cage-cleaning

46

regimes should minimise noise, dissemination of unfamiliar conspecific odours, and

47

physical disturbance during very late pregnancy and the first few days following birth.

48

Keywords: animal welfare; husbandry; hygiene; cannibalism; reproduction; rodents

2

49

1.

50

Cleaning of animals’ cages or enclosures is necessary for almost all captive animals,

51

whether they are kept in laboratories, farms, or zoos, or as pets, to prevent waste

52

products accumulating to harmful or aversive levels. More frequent cleaning of rodent

53

cages can reduce ammonia levels (Carissimi et al., 2000; Reeb-Whitaker et al., 2001;

54

Burn et al., 2006a) and microbial counts (Borrello et al., 1998), and increase rodent

55

health (e.g. Cisar & Jayson, 1967; Van Winkle & Balk, 1986). However, cage-

56

cleaning almost always involves temporary displacement of each animal, physical and

57

olfactory disruption of the ‘home’ environment, and human contact, which could each

58

cause stress. Cage-cleaning does indeed cause acute increases in arousal and possibly

59

transient stress in diverse species including rhesus macaques (Line et al., 1989), mice

60

(Gray & Hurst, 1995; but see Blom et al., 1993), hamsters (Conn et al., 1990;

61

Gattermann & Weinandy, 1996), rats (Saibaba et al., 1996; Schnecko et al., 1998;

62

Duke et al., 2001; Sharp et al., 2002; Burn et al., 2006b) and snakes (Chiszar et al.,

63

1980).

64

Introduction

Whether frequent cage-cleaning causes chronic or cumulative stress is far less

65

certain. In mice, two studies found that cleaning mouse cages more frequently

66

increases pup mortality (Chantry, 1964; Reeb-Whitaker et al., 2001). Breeding mice

67

whose cages were cleaned more frequently also tended to have higher corticosterone

68

concentrations (Reeb-Whitaker et al., 2001). Even merely disturbing breeding mice,

69

by regularly lifting the cage-lid and inspecting the hidden individuals, caused a strong

70

trend towards poorer breeding success (Peters et al., 2002).

71 72

In our own previous studies, we investigated whether frequent cage-cleaning affected the welfare of male experimental Wistar and Sprague-Dawley rats, and found

3

73

little evidence that it is a cause for concern in those animals (Burn, 2006; Burn et al.,

74

2006a; Burn et al., 2006b). However, one small-scale study of breeding rats did find

75

indications that cage-cleaning has long-term effects. That work involved six Osborne-

76

Mendel females per treatment (Cisar & Jayson, 1967). More pups were successfully

77

weaned, they weighed more, and fewer needed to be culled by the experimenters in

78

the twice-weekly cage-cleaned group than those in the weekly cleaned group.

79

However, cannibalism was more frequent in the twice-weekly cleaned group.

80

Our aim here was to investigate how cage-cleaning frequency affects the

81

general health, welfare, and reproductive success of breeding rats and their pups

82

on a large scale. Even if non-breeding rats are little affected by frequent-cage-

83

cleaning (Burn, 2006; Burn et al., 2006a; Burn et al., 2006b), we were concerned that

84

breeding rat populations could be more affected, for a number of reasons. Firstly,

85

olfactory signals are very important in rat parental care. These include the pheromone,

86

diodecyl proprionate, produced from the pup preputial gland to induce maternal

87

licking (Brouettelahlou et al., 1991); odours produced by dams that prevent cohabiting

88

males from killing pups (Mennella & Moltz, 1988), and that guide pups to the nipples

89

(Porter & Winberg, 1999); and scents deposited in the bedding by the dams that

90

reduce pup activity levels, keeping them in the nest (Porter & Winberg, 1999). Cage-

91

cleaning could directly remove some of those signals (Mennella & Moltz, 1988;

92

Porter & Winberg, 1999), and/or mask them with odours from human hands or gloves

93

(including scents from previously handled rats). Furthermore, rodent cage-cleaning is

94

accompanied by loud noise (Gamble, 1982; Voipio et al., 2006), physical

95

displacement, human contact, and exposure to relatively bright light (Lane Petter,

96

1968; Busnel & Molin, 1978; Libbin & Person, 1979). It can also physically disrupt

4

97

the nest structure, and involves transferring pups to a temporarily colder environment

98

(e.g. Chantry, 1964; Lee & Williams, 1975). Acute physical stressors such as these

99

could trigger neglect and/or cannibalism of pups (Lane Petter, 1968). In the longer-

100

term, if frequent cage-cleaning causes chronic stress to the parents, this could also

101

reduce their fertility (Boice, 1972), reduce pup birth weights and increase mortality

102

rates (e.g. Cabrera et al., 1999).

103

Rats comprise 20% of all laboratory animals used in Europe (Commission of

104

the European Communities, 2003) and about 14% of those in Canada (Canadian

105

Council on Animal Care, 2001); thus any findings relevant to their health, welfare,

106

and productivity would have implications for a large number of animals, in both

107

commercial and research facilities. Here, we assessed the effects of twice-weekly,

108

weekly and fortnightly cage-cleaning on the rats of a commercial breeding company

109

(Harlan, Bicester, UK). We used outbred albino Wistars, chosen because they are one

110

of the most commonly used stocks in the UK, and because they are pair-mated (rather

111

than harem-mated), enabling us to follow the lifetime breeding performance of

112

individual pairs. Because of the biosecurity requirements of the commercial

113

establishment, all measurements (except chromodacryorrhoea) were carried out

114

by the animals’ normal technicians. Therefore, detailed behavioural observations

115

were unfeasible, but measurements included as many rapidly observable aspects

116

of rat health and welfare as possible.

5

117

118

2.

Materials and methods 2.1.

Animals and housing

119

The study animals were 108 pairs of Wistar (HsdBrlHan:WIST) rats, consisting of 54

120

pairs housed in each of two similar buildings (‘A’ and ‘B’) maintained under

121

commercial, barrier conditions. The cages were polypropylene (51 x 32 x 20 cm, L x

122

W x H) with a stainless steel mesh lid. Each cage contained autoclaved Lignocel 3-4

123

sawdust (J. Rettenmaier & Söhne, Holzmühle, Germany), to a depth of about 2 cm.

124

They were provided with pelleted chow (2018S sterilisable, Harlan Teklad, Bicester,

125

UK) ad libitum, and water was constantly available from an automatic system. The

126

temperature and humidity were 19-21oC and 55 +/- 1 %, respectively. The light:dark

127

cycle was 12:12 h, with 2 h dawn and dusk periods (only half the lights were on).

128

In Building A there were approximately 6200 adult rats plus pre-weanling

129

offspring (BN/SsN, LE and LH) and in Building B there were 6000 (all

130

HsdBrlHan:WIST). Obvious differences between the buildings were that the

131

colony in A was populated in 2001 and all rats were kept in polypropylene cages,

132

while B was populated in 2004 and rats outside the current study were kept in

133

wire-bottomed cages. The individual staff also differed between the buildings.

134

The study was staggered between the two buildings, with Building A being 1 month

135

ahead of Building B. Pairs were formed at 10 weeks of age. The experiment continued

136

for the commercial lifetimes of the pairs, which is 9 months (36 weeks). Pups were

137

weaned between 19 and 26 days of age (depending on their weight) at cage-

138

cleaning, as is standard practice.

139 140

Within each building, cages were allocated to the following cage-cleaning regimes: twice-weekly, once-weekly, or every two weeks – ‘fortnightly’ (n = 18 pairs 6

141

per treatment in each building). Cages were cleaned by replacing the cage bases and

142

bedding with fresh ones, but the cage lids were retained. Cage emptying was carried

143

out manually on an unventilated table. The rats were always handled by the

144

same technician. Due to the scale of the technicians’ work in this commercial

145

facility, randomising the positions of cages or the cleaning sequence proved to be

146

logistically unmanageable, so cages were instead arranged within same-treatment

147

racks. The tier-positions of cages within the racks, which are known to affect rat

148

behaviour and physiology (e.g. Rao, 1991; Reinhardt, 2004; Izídio et al., 2005), were

149

balanced across treatments, but the replication across the two buildings was especially

150

worthwhile to help control for unknown rack effects.

151

The animals were cared for in accordance with the code of practice for the

152

housing and care of animals in designated breeding and supplying establishments

153

(Home Office, 1995), and was approved by the local ethical review process at the

154

University of Oxford.

155

2.2.

Data collection

156

The breeding females (in Building A only) were weighed at the beginning and end of

157

the study. Breeding parameters were measured, including the number of pups born in

158

each litter, the number and sexes of the pups at each weaning, and for the first and

159

third parity the body weights of one male and one female pup. The ages of the pups

160

when cage-cleaning first occurred was noted for each litter. The rate of reproductive

161

decline of the mothers was measured using the total number of litters they produced,

162

and also as the ratio of the number of pups in their ninth litter (80% of the pairs

163

produced at least 9 litters) against the number in their second litter (the first one often

164

being atypical). Adult and pup mortalities were noted, and the causes of pup death 7

165

were recorded as ‘stillborn’, ‘eaten by the parents’, ‘missing’, ‘found dead’ or, in the

166

case of cage-flooding, ‘wet’.

167

After 4 months in each building, ammonia concentrations were measured in

168

each cage. A pump with Gastec glass measuring tubes with a range of 0.5–60 ppm

169

were used (Anachem Ltd., Bedfordshire, UK). These were inserted in through the

170

door in the top of the cage, and the measurements were taken in the centre of the cage

171

just above the sawdust. Measurements were taken on the day before all cages were

172

cleaned, so the fortnightly cleaned cages had 13 days of soiling, contrasting with the

173

twice-weekly cleaned ones, which had only 2–3 days of soiling.

174

Both parents’ noses were scored for chromodacryorrhoea – ‘red/bloody tears’,

175

a Harderian gland secretion that increases in a variety of aversive situations in rats

176

(Harkness & Ridgway, 1980; Mason et al., 2004; Burn, 2006) – by an experienced

177

experimenter, wearing a powered respirator due to rodent allergy. This took place 8

178

months into the project because older rats produce more chromodacryorrhoea than

179

young ones (Harkness & Ridgway, 1980), and hence the secretion is more visible.

180

Scoring was carried out in Building B alone, because it was not possible to enter both

181

buildings within the necessary timescale due to potential disease transfer (particularly

182

since the respirator could not be completely sterilised). The observer tapped gently

183

with a pen on the front of each cage to encourage the rats to present their noses for

184

inspection. The method used for scoring was on a 6-point scale (0-5), as described

185

elsewhere (Burn et al., 2006a; Burn et al., 2006b). Chromodacryorrhoea

186

measurements were taken the morning before cage-cleaning, when the soiling of the

187

different cages was at its most contrasting, and again at the same time on the day of

188

cleaning (about 2 h after cage-cleaning).

8

189

2.3.

Statistical analysis

190

For the lifetime breeding parameters and ammonia measurements, general linear

191

models (GLMs) were used, with cage-cleaning frequency and building, and their

192

interactions as fixed factors. For the pup weights from the first and third litters, the

193

sex of the pups and their cage (a random factor, nested in cage-cleaning frequency and

194

building) were also included. For chromodacryorrhoea, the cage, the order in which

195

cages were observed, and whether the measurement was taken before or after cleaning

196

were included alongside cleaning frequency, and their interactions. Data were square-

197

root, log, or arcsine square-root transformed as necessary, and the model residuals

198

were inspected graphically to verify whether they met the assumptions of the model.

199

For those variables that did not meet the assumptions of GLMs, logistic regressions

200

were used. Models were improved by removing redundant non-significant

201

interactions. The statistical program used was MinitabTM version 14 (Minitab Ltd,

202

Pennsylvania, USA).

203

To assess effects on cannibalism, pups that were known to have been cannibalised,

204

which is rarely witnessed directly, and those who were noted as ‘missing’, were

205

combined into a single category (similar to Mohan, 1974). The effect of the age of the

206

pups at their first cage-cleaning on the likelihood of cannibalism was analysed using a

207

Generalized Linear Mixed Model (glmmPQL, MASS library, R freeware, version

208

2.4); cannibalism was the binary response, and the treatment group, mean age at first

209

cage-cleaning, mean litter size, and their interactions were the predictors. Cage

210

(nested within the treatment group) was included as a random factor.

9

211

3.

212

Cage-cleaning frequency had no significant main effects except that more pups were

213

cannibalised when cages were cleaned more frequently (Figure 1). Other recorded

214

mortalities did not show this pattern. More frequent cleaning increased cannibalism

215

in terms of both how many pups were eaten or missing (z = 2.15; n = 106; P = 0.032)

216

and the percentage of pairs that did or did not eat or miss pups (z = 2.06; n = 106; P =

217

0.039). An interaction between age at first cage-cleaning and the treatment group

218

meant that, in the twice-weekly and, to a lesser extent, the once-weekly cleaning

219

treatments cannibalism was more likely to occur if cages were cleaned sooner after

220

birth (i.e. when the pups were younger), but this was not observed in the fortnightly

221

treatment (t = 2.85, DF = 882, with 106 clusters, P = 0.005) (Figure 2). The absolute

222

percentage of pups that were cannibalised was only 2.60 +/- 0.04 % of all pups born,

223

but at least one pup was cannibalised from 10.9% of litters. Also, cannibalism was the

224

largest recorded type of pup mortality (stillborns: 0.38% of pups born; individuals

225

found dead: 0.77%; and deaths from cage-flooding: 0.76%). The weaning weights of

226

surviving pups from litters that had included some cannibalised pups did not

227

differ significantly from other pup weights.

228

Results

Cage-cleaning frequency had no other main effects, not even on ammonia

229

concentrations (twice-weekly mean +/- S.E.: 13.5+/-0.5 versus fortnightly: 13.9+/-0.6;

230

P = 0.642). However, it interacted with the building the rats were housed in (Figure

231

3): the fortnightly cleaning group had the fewest pups born and weaned in Building A,

232

but in Building B the most were born and weaned in that treatment group (number

233

born: F2, 100 = 4.47; P = 0.014; number weaned: F2, 100 = 4.30; P = 0.016). This effect

234

was also apparent for the mean litter sizes, with rats in fortnightly cleaned cages

10

235

having the smallest litters in Building A, and the largest in Building B (F2, 100 = 7.86;

236

P = 0.001) (Figure 3).

237

Significantly more births were recorded on the cage-cleaning day itself,

238

compared with non-cleaning days (F1, 107 = 37.79; P < 0.001) (Figure 4). Thus, births

239

on the day of cage-cleaning appeared around twice as likely as expected by chance, in

240

the weekly and fortnightly cleaning groups. For the fortnightly cleaning treatment,

241

Figure 4 suggests that births also peaked one week after cleaning.

242

Building also had a significant main effect (Table 1) on every variable tested,

243

except for the pup sex ratios and mortality from cannibalism or stillbirths (parental

244

chromodacryorrhoea and maternal weight were only tested in one building). In

245

Building A more pups were born and weaned and there were more litters in total, but

246

each pup was lighter than in Building B and a higher proportion of them were found

247

dead. The weight difference between male and female pups (males being heavier) was

248

significantly more pronounced in Building B than in A for the first litters.

249

4.

250

We investigated whether cage-cleaning frequency affected the reproductive

251

performance and welfare of breeding rats. Overall, we found no significant main

252

effects of cleaning frequency on any health and fertility measures (mortality rates and

253

numbers of pups born and weaned), on other indices sensitive to stress

254

(chromodacryorrhoea, maternal reproductive decline and weight gain, and pup sex

255

ratio), or on ammonia levels. Furthermore, in a follow-up study, we found no

256

significant effects on pup handleability or anxiety profiles in adulthood (Burn et al., in

257

press).

Discussion

11

258

However, we found that cannibalism increased with more frequent cage-

259

cleaning, which is consistent with the small-scale study by Cisar and Jayson (1967),

260

and with common perception (e.g. Home Office, 1995; Hansen et al., 2000).

261

Cannibalism was rare, affecting about 2.6% of all the pups born, which is

262

approximately consistent with previous studies (Chantry, 1964; Reynolds, 1981;

263

DeSantis & Schmaltz, 1984), so cleaning frequency had no significant effect on pup

264

mortality generally. Nevertheless, under these clean conditions and with this outbred

265

strain, other causes of mortality were even rarer. Therefore, on a large scale or with

266

more susceptible rat strains, reducing cannibalism might lead to signifcant increases

267

in the numbers of pups weaned.

268

Cannibalistic behaviour is not a uniform phenomenon (Elwood, 1991); it can

269

sometimes be a response to pups that are already dead or dying, and other times be a

270

by-product of direct infanticide, which in turn can be a response to various different

271

situations (Fox, 1975; DeSantis & Schmaltz, 1984). Here we found no indication that

272

it occurred as a result of poor maternal health or welfare (Boice, 1972), since cage-

273

cleaning frequency had no significant effects on maternal chromodacryorrhoea,

274

weight gain, rate of reproductive decline, or litter sex ratios. Cage-cleaning also did

275

not seem to affect litter size, so the increased cannibalism in the more frequent cage-

276

cleaning groups was not a response to larger litters (Day & Galef, 1977; Gandelman

277

& Simon, 1978).

278

It is possible that cannibalism was a response to a higher proportion of weak

279

pups in the more frequent cleaning groups; for example, if the cleaning process itself

280

triggered birth (Figure 4), this could have resulted in a higher proportion of premature

281

litters. Alternatively, (or in addition) frequent cage-cleaning might have increased

12

282

cannibalism through more frequent olfactory (e.g. Mennella & Moltz, 1988; Moles et

283

al., 2004) auditory, and/or physical (Lane Petter, 1968; Busnel & Molin, 1978)

284

disturbances, or nest disruption and cooling (e.g. Chantry, 1964; Lee & Williams,

285

1975) at a time when pups were vulnerable. Noise certainly reaches a peak during

286

cleaning and is a stressor (Gamble, 1982; Voipio et al., 2006), so cleaning cages as

287

quietly as possible around breeding rats could help prevent cannibalism (Lane

288

Petter, 1968; Busnel & Molin, 1978). Cage-cleaning in an unventilated area of

289

the stock room also increases airborne levels of rodent urinary proteins (Thulin

290

et al., 2002), potentially disseminating unfamiliar conspecific odours between

291

cages – since unfamiliar male odours are known to stress mother rats (Moles et

292

al., 2004), this olfactory disturbance should be avoided where possible. Figure 2

293

shows that, in more frequently cleaned cages, cannibalism was more likely to occur

294

if cage-cleaning occurred on the day of birth (consistent with cannibalism of

295

prematurely born pups), or during the first 2 days following it (consistent with

296

cannibalism when cages containing very young pups are disturbed). Note that we had

297

no information on when cannibalism events occurred, so even though cage-cleaning

298

affected them, they were not necessarily on the same day as cage-cleaning.

299

In fact, the vast majority of cannibalism in rats has been reported to occur

300

during the first week after birth, with some taking place in the second week, and

301

virtually none in the third week (DeSantis & Schmaltz, 1984). Some rodent breeders

302

already avoid cage-cleaning between birth and weaning (Libbin & Person, 1979), or

303

at least within the first few days of birth (Peters et al., 2002), specifically to prevent

304

cannibalism and neglect by the parents. Our data suggest that for twice-weekly and

13

305

possibly once-weekly cleaning regimes this might be an effective way of reducing

306

cannibalism (Figure 2).

307

Overall, cage-cleaning frequency affected the mean litter size, and

308

correspondingly the numbers of pups born and weaned, but the effect depended on the

309

building the pups were born and raised in: fortnightly cage-cleaning was associated

310

with the greatest number of pups in one building and the fewest in the other. These

311

interactions between cage-cleaning frequency and building imply that if we had only

312

carried out our study in Building A, we would have concluded that fortnightly cage-

313

cleaning reduces the numbers of pups, while we would have concluded the opposite

314

had we only used Building B. This could have been due to differences between the

315

cage racks, the different humans working in the buildings, olfactory or vocal

316

communications within the different animal populations, or any number of

317

environmental differences. This supports and extends the finding that rats of the same

318

strain differ between different breeding companies (Rex et al., 1996; Germann et al.,

319

1998); here they differed within the same breeding company. Therefore, in agreement

320

with previous studies (e.g. Crabbe et al., 1999; Wurbel, 2000; Wahlsten et al., 2003;

321

Garner et al., 2006), our findings reveal difficulties regarding the feasibility of

322

standardisation between different experiments, and reaffirm the merits of replicating

323

research under different conditions.

324

Unlike Cisar and Jayson (1967), we did not find that overall weaning success

325

was increased by more frequent cage-cleaning. This might be explained by the

326

relative contribution of hygiene to pup survival in the two studies. In Cisar and

327

Jayson’s study, rats were kept in a conventional animal unit in the 1960’s, when

328

around 97.5% of pup mortality in similar units was due to diseases (Porter, 1968).

14

329

Therefore, frequent cage-cleaning might have been essential for preventing disease. In

330

contrast, our study animals were kept in a barrier unit, in which there were no detected

331

pathogens (Harlan’s Health Monitoring reports, 2005, unpublished) and ammonia

332

levels were almost certainly lower (Table 1, cf. Perkins & Lipman, 1995; Carissimi et

333

al., 2000).

334

Here, the cage-cleaning days themselves were associated with more births than

335

non-cleaning days. This may have been an artefact of the way that births were

336

recorded: pup births and ages were usually recorded and estimated during cage-

337

cleaning, so very young pups might have been recorded as being born that day

338

unless it was obvious that they were several days old. However, the opinions of

339

the staff involved differed over whether this recording artefact would occur or

340

not, so other explanations require consideration. Also, a recording artefact

341

would not fully explain the peak in births midway through the cleaning cycle in

342

the fortnightly treatment. An alternative explanation could be that the noise

343

(Lane Petter, 1968; Busnel & Molin, 1978), circulation of conspecific odours

344

(Moles et al., 2004), or physical disturbance associated with cleaning, even of

345

neighbouring cages, triggered births perhaps through stress or increased activity

346

in the mother. Physical strain, and chronic and acute stress, can shorten gestational

347

duration under some circumstances in humans (Glynn et al., 2001; Hobel & Culhane,

348

2003) and domestic animals (Silver, 1990), but the effects are little understood, and

349

differ between species. The mid-way peak in the fortnightly treatment also suggests

350

that we may not have entirely effectively simulated the fortnightly cleaning rate, when

351

all cages in a room would be cleaned fortnightly.

15

352

Apart from cage-cleaning effects, most variables differed between the two

353

buildings. Interestingly, the differences between the buildings corresponded to the

354

pattern of reproductive trade-offs predicted by theory, in that offspring ‘quality’ is

355

balanced against the number of offspring produced (Smith & Fretwell, 1974).

356

Building A produced more offspring (in terms of numbers born, weaned and the

357

number of litters), but each individual pup weighed less, sexual dimorphism was

358

reduced, and pup mortalities were higher than in Building B.

359

5.

Conclusion

360

Here and in a follow up study (Burn et al., in press), we found no clear

361

benefits of frequent cage-cleaning to breeding rats, but it did increase the likelihood of

362

cannibalism, and the cleaning process itself might have triggered parturition. We

363

would therefore recommend that cages are not cleaned during the last few days of

364

pregnancy (possibly avoiding premature induction of birth) or the first 2 days

365

following birth (avoiding acute early disturbances). Also, more research is required

366

about what aspects of cleaning trigger cannibalism, but until then, noise and the

367

transfer of odours between cages are likely stressors, and should be kept to a

368

minimum when cleaning the cages of breeding rats.

369

6.

370

Many thanks to Alan Peters for his practical advice and expertise, and the staff at

371

Harlan who made this study possible. Robert Campbell and Bill Browne are also

372

gratefully acknowledged for their help with the GLMM and Figure 2. This work was

373

funded by the UK Home Office, on the recommendation of the Animal Procedures

374

Committee.

Acknowledgements

16

375

7.

376

Blom, H. J. M., Witkam, A. C. P., Schlingmann, F., Hoogervorst, M. J. C., Van de

377

Weerd, H. A., Baumans, V., Beynen, A. C., 1993. Demonstration of

378

preference for clean versus soiled cages as expressed by laboratory mice.

379

Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.

380 381 382

References

Boice, R., 1972. Some behavioral tests of domestication in Norway rats. Behaviour 42, 198-231. Borrello, P., D'Amore, E., Di Virgilio, A., Panzini, G., Valeri, M., Lorenzini, R. N.,

383

1998. The use of AGI as a method to measure microbial contamination in the

384

cage microenvironment. Anim. Technol. 49, 81-85.

385

Brouettelahlou, I., Amouroux, R., Chastrette, F., Cosnier, J., Stoffelsma, J.,

386

Vernetmaury, E., 1991. Dodecyl propionate, attractant from rat pup preputial

387

gland - characterization and identification. J. Chem. Ecol. 17, 1343-1354.

388

Burn, C. C., 2006. Effects of husbandry manipulations and the laboratory

389

environment on rat health and welfare. Unpublished D.Phil. thesis, University

390

of Oxford, Oxford, UK.

391

Burn, C. C., Day, M. J., Peters, A., Mason, G. J., 2006a. Long-term effects of cage-

392

cleaning frequency and bedding type on laboratory rat health, welfare, and

393

handleability: a cross-laboratory study. Lab. Anim. 40, 353-370.

394

Burn, C. C., Peters, A., Mason, G. J., 2006b. Acute effects of cage-cleaning at

395

different frequencies on laboratory rat behaviour and welfare. Anim. Welf. 15,

396

161-172.

17

397

Burn, C. C., Deacon, R., Mason, G. J., in press. Marked for life? Effects of early cage

398

cleaning frequency, delivery batch and identification tail-marking on adult rat

399

anxiety profiles. Dev. Psychobiol.

400

Busnel, M. C., Molin, D., 1978. Preliminary results of the effects of noise on gestating

401

mice and their pups. In J. L. Fletch, R. G. Busnel (Eds.), Effects of noise on

402

wildlife (pp. 209-248). New York: Academic Press.

403

Cabrera, R. J., Rodriguez-Echandia, E. L., Jatuff, A. S., Foscolo, M., 1999. Effects of

404

prenatal exposure to a mild chronic variable stress on body weight,

405

preweaning mortality and rat behavior. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 32, 1229-

406

1237.

407

Canadian Council on Animal Care. (2001). CCAC Animal Use Survey - 2001.

408

Retrieved July 2007, from

409

http://www.ccac.ca/en/Publications/Facts_Figures/pdfs/aus2001-all.pdf

410

Carissimi, A. S., Chaguri, L. C. A. A., Teixeira, M. A., Mori, C. M. C., Macchione,

411

M., Sant' Anna, E. T. G., Saldiva, P. H. N., Souza, N. L., Merusse, J. B. L.,

412

2000. Effects of two ventilation systems and bedding change frequency on

413

cage environmental factors in rats (Rattus norvegicus). Anim. Technol. 51,

414

161-170.

415 416 417

Chantry, D., 1964. The effects of handling and cage changing on breeding mice. J. Anim. Technicians Assoc. 15, 78-80. Chiszar, D., Wellborn, S., Wand, M. A., Scudder, K. M., Smith, H. M., 1980.

418

Investigatory behavior in snakes, 2: cage cleaning and the defecation in

419

snakes. Anim. Learn. Behav. 8, 505-510.

18

420 421

Cisar, C. F., Jayson, G., 1967. Effects of frequency of cage cleaning on rat litters prior to weaning. Lab. Anim. Care 17, 215-217.

422

Commission of the European Communities. 2003. Third report from the Commission

423

to the Council and the European Parliament on the statistics on the number of

424

animals used for experimental and other scientific purposes in the member

425

states of the European Union (No. COM/2003/0019). Brussels: Commission

426

of the European Communities.

427

Conn, C. A., Borer, K. T., Kluger, M. J., 1990. Body temperature rhythm and

428

response to pyrogen in exercising and sedentary hamsters. Med. Sci. Sports

429

Exerc. 22, 636-642.

430 431 432 433 434

Crabbe, J. C., Wahlsten, D., Dudek, B. C., 1999. Genetics of mouse behavior: interactions with laboratory environment. Science 284, 1670-1672. Day, C. S., Galef, B. G., 1977. Pup cannibalism: One aspect of maternal behavior in golden hamsters. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 91, 1179-1189. DeSantis, D. T., Schmaltz, L. W., 1984. The mother-litter relationship in

435

developmental rat studies: cannibalism vs caring. Dev. Psychobiol. 17, 255-

436

262.

437

Duke, J., Zammit, T., Lawson, D., 2001. The effects of routine cage-cleaning on

438

cardiovascular and behavioural parameters in male Sprague-Dawley rats.

439

Contemp. Top. Lab. Anim. Sci. 40, 17-20.

440 441 442 443

Elwood, R. W., 1991. Ethical implications of studies on infanticide and maternal aggression in rodents. Anim. Behav. 42, 841-849. Fox, L. R., 1975. Cannibalism in natural populations. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 6, 87106.

19

444 445 446 447

Gamble, M. R., 1982. Noise and laboratory animals. J. Inst. Anim. Technicians 33, 515. Gandelman, R., Simon, N. G., 1978. Spontaneous pup-killing by mice in response to large litters. Dev. Psychobiol. 11, 235-241.

448

Garner, J. P., Thogerson, C. M., Mench, J. A., Würbel, H., 2006. Standardization and

449

the Red Queen: applying methodologies from ethology, neuropsychology, and

450

field biology to problems in high-throughput behavioral methods. Paper

451

presented at the Proceedings of the 40th International Congress of the ISAE,

452

Bristol.

453

Gattermann, R., Weinandy, R., 1996. Time of day and stress response to different

454

stressors in experimental animals. Part I: Golden hamster (Mesocricetus

455

auratus Waterhouse, 1839). J. Exp. Anim. Sci. 38, 66-76.

456

Germann, P. G., Hafner, D., Hanauer, G., Drommer, W., 1998. Incidence and severity

457

of granulomatous pneumonia in Brown Norway rats: Breeder related

458

variations. J. Exp. Anim. Sci. 39, 22-33.

459

Glynn, L. M., Wadhwa, P. D., Dunkel-Schetter, C., Chicz-DeMet, A., Sandman, C.

460

A., 2001. When stress happens matters: Effects of earthquake timing on stress

461

responsivity in pregnancy. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 184, 637-642.

462 463 464

Gray, S., Hurst, J. L., 1995. The effect of cage cleaning on aggression within groups of male laboratory mice. Anim. Behav. 49, 821-826. Hansen, A. K., Baumans, V., Elliott, H., Francis, R., Holgate, B., Hubrecht, R.,

465

Jennings, M., Peters, A., Stauffacher, M., 2000. Future principles for housing

466

and care of laboratory rodents and rabbits (second meeting). Strasbourg:

467

Report of the Working Party for the Preparation of the Fourth Multilateral

20

468

Consultation of Parties to the European Convention for the Protection of

469

Vertebrate Animals used for Experimental and Other Scientific Purposes.

470 471 472 473 474

Harkness, J. E., Ridgway, M. D., 1980. Chromodacryorrhea in laboratory rats (Rattus norvegicus): etiologic considerations. Lab. Anim. Sci. 30, 841-844. Hobel, C., Culhane, J., 2003. Role of psychosocial and nutritional stress on poor pregnancy outcome. J. Nutr. 133, 1709S-1717. Home Office, 1995. Code of Practice for the housing and care of animals in

475

designated breeding and supplying establishments, Home Office, UK

476

Izídio, G. S., Lopes, D. M., Spricigo Jr, L., Ramos, A., 2005. Common variations in

477

the pretest environment influence genotypic comparisons in models of anxiety.

478

Genes, Brain & Behavior 4, 412-419.

479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487

Lane Petter, W., 1968. Cannibalism in rats and mice. Proc. R. Soc. Med. 61, 12951296. Lee, M. H. S., Williams, D. I., 1975. Long term changes in nest condition and pup grouping following handling of rat litters. Dev. Psychobiol. 8, 91-95. Libbin, R. M., Person, P., 1979. Neonatal rat surgery: Avoiding maternal cannibalism. Science 206, 66. Line, S. W., Morgan, K. N., Markowitz, H., 1989. Heart rate and activity of rhesus monkeys in response to routine events. Lab. Primate Newsl. 28, 9-12. Mason, G., Wilson, D., Hampton, C., Wurbel, H., 2004. Non-invasively assessing

488

disturbance and stress in laboratory rats by scoring chromodacryorrhoea. Alt.

489

Lab. Anim. 32, 153-159.

490 491

Mennella, J. A., Moltz, H., 1988. Infanticide in rats: male strategy and female counter-strategy. Physiol. Behav. 42, 19-28.

21

492 493

Mohan, C., 1974. Age-dependent cannibalism in a colony of albino rats. Lab. Anim. 8, 83-84.

494

Moles, A., Rizzi, R., D'Amato, F. R., 2004. Postnatal stress in mice: Does stressing

495

the mother have the same effect as stressing the pups? Dev. Psychobiol. 44,

496

230-237.

497

Perkins, S. E., Lipman, N. S., 1995. Characterization and quantification of

498

microenvironmental contaminants in isolator cages with a variety of contact

499

beddings. Contemp. Top. Lab. Anim. Sci. 34, 93-98.

500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508

Peters, A. G., Bywater, P. M., Festing, M. F. W., 2002. The effect of daily disturbance on the breeding performance of mice. Lab. Anim. 36, 188-192. Porter, G., 1968. Pre-weaning loss of laboratory animals. Proc. R. Soc. Med. 61, 1295. Porter, R. H., Winberg, J., 1999. Unique salience of maternal breast odors for newborn infants. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 439-449. Rao, G. N., 1991. Light intensity-associated eye lesions of Fischer-344 rats in longterm studies. Toxicol. Pathol. 19, 148-155. Reeb-Whitaker, C. K., Paigen, B., Beamer, W. G., Bronson, R. T., Churchill, G. A.,

509

Schweitzer, I. B., Myers, D. D., 2001. The impact of reduced frequency of

510

cage changes on the health of mice housed in ventilated cages. Lab. Anim. 35,

511

58-73.

512 513

Reinhardt, V., 2004. Common husbandry-related variables in biomedical research with animals. Lab. Anim. 38, 213-235.

514

Rex, A., Sondern, U., Voigt, J. P., Franck, S., Fink, H., 1996. Strain differences in

515

fear-motivated behavior of rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 54, 107-111.

22

516

Reynolds, R. D., 1981. Preventing maternal cannibalism in rats. Science 213, 1146.

517

Saibaba, P., Sales, G. D., Stodulski, G., Hau, J., 1996. Behaviour of rats in their home

518

cages: Daytime variations and effects of routine husbandry procedures

519

analysed by time sampling techniques. Lab. Anim. 30, 13-21.

520

Schnecko, A., Witte, K., Lemmer, B., 1998. Effects of routine procedures on

521

cardiovascular parameters of Sprague-Dawley rats in periods of activity and

522

rest. J. Exp. Anim. Sci. 38, 181-190.

523

Sharp, J. L., Zammit, T. G., Azar, T. A., Lawson, D. M., 2002. Stress-like responses

524

to common procedures in male rats housed alone or with other rats. Contemp.

525

Top. Lab. Anim. Sci. 41, 8-14.

526 527 528 529 530

Silver, M., 1990. Prenatal maturation, the timing of birth and how it may be regulated in domestic animals. Exp. Physiol. 75, 285-307. Smith, C. C., Fretwell, S. D., 1974. Optimal balance between size and number of offspring. Am. Nat. 108, 499-506. Thulin, H., Bjorkdahl, M., Karlsson, A.-s., Renstrom, A., 2002. Reduction of

531

exposure to laboratory animal allergens in a research laboratory. Ann. Occup.

532

Hyg. 46, 61-68.

533 534 535

Van Winkle, T. J., Balk, M. W., 1986. Spontaneous corneal opacities in laboratory mice. Lab. Anim. Sci. 36, 248-255. Voipio, H. M., Nevalainen, T., Halonen, P., Hakumäki, M., Björk, E., 2006. Role of

536

cage material, working style and hearing sensitivity in perception of animal

537

care noise. Lab. Anim. 40, 400-409.

538 539

Wahlsten, D., Metten, P., Phillips, T. J., Boehm, S. L., Burkhart-Kasch, S., Dorow, J., Doerksen, S., Downing, C., Fogarty, J., Rodd-Henricks, K., Hen, R.,

23

540

McKinnon, C. S., Merrill, C. M., Nolte, C., Schalomon, M., Schlumbohm, J.

541

P., Sibert, J. R., Wenger, C. D., Dudek, B. C., Crabbe, J. C., 2003. Different

542

data from different labs: Lessons from studies of gene-environment

543

interaction. J. Neurobiol. 54, 283-311.

544

Wurbel, H., 2000. Behaviour and the standardization fallacy. Nat. Genet. 26, 263.

545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562

24

563

Figure and table captions

564

Figure 1. The effect of cage-cleaning frequency on cannibalism of pups. More

565

frequent cleaning increased (A) the mean (+/- S.E.) proportion of pups that were

566

cannibalised in the two buildings, and (B) the percentage of parents that cannibalised

567

pups at some stage during the project. * indicates the pairwise significant difference

568

(P < 0.05) on graph A, but in graph B it was the covariate trend across cleaning

569

intervals that was significant.

570

Figure 2. The relationship between the age of the litters when their cages were first

571

cleaned and the likelihood of cannibalism. The fitted lines and shaded 95%

572

confidence intervals show that for the twice-weekly, and to a lesser extent the once-

573

weekly, cleaning group cannibalism was more likely if cage-cleaning occurred earlier

574

in life. There was no significant trend in the fortnightly cleaned group. Note that the

575

day of cleaning might not necessarily have been the day the pups were cannibalised;

576

data on when the cannibalism occurred were not available, so the pups may have been

577

eaten at a later date.

578

Figure 3. The mean (S.E.) lifetime number of pups (A) born and (B) weaned, and (C)

579

the litter size for breeding pairs cage-cleaned at different frequencies in two different

580

buildings. The fortnightly cage-cleaning decreased the number of offspring compared

581

with more frequent cage-cleaning in Building A, but it increased the number of

582

offspring in Building B.

583

Figure 4. The numbers of pups born on each day of the (A) twice-weekly, (B) weekly

584

and (C) fortnightly cage-cleaning cycle. The cage-cleaning day, Day 0, is

585

highlighted in black. The dotted lines show the numbers of pups expected by chance

586

if they were distributed evenly across days. For the twice-weekly group there is a step

25

587

in probability, because half the cleaning events occurred on Day 2 of the cycle

588

(Fridays) and half on Day 3 (Tuesdays), so pups had 50% less chance of being born

589

on Day 3 than any other day. In general more births were recorded on the cage-

590

cleaning day itself than on other days, but the fortnightly cleaning group also suggests

591

a peak midway through the cleaning cycle, on the day when all neighbouring cages

592

would have been cleaned.

593

Table 1. Significant differences between the two buildings. The statistics used were

594

either GLMs (where an F-value is given), or for non-parametric data, ordinal logistic

595

regressions (where a z-value is given). In Building A, significantly more pups were

596

born and weaned, and there were more litters in total, but pups were lighter and more

597

frequently found dead than in Building B. Ammonia concentrations were low in both

598

buildings, but were higher in B than in A. Maternal weight and parental

599

chromodacryorrhoea could not be compared between the buildings because

600

measurements were only possible in one building.

601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610

26

611

Table 1

Variable

Statistical values

Number born per

F1, 100 = 6.40; P =

pair

0.013

Number weaned

F1, 100 = 4.70; P =

per pair

0.032

Number of litters

Odds = 3.59; z =

per pair

3.39; n = 106; P =

Building A

Building B

Effect

(mean +/- SE)

(mean +/- SE) direction

98.9 +/- 3.4

87.6 +/- 3.0

A>B

92.1 +/- 3.0

82.9 +/- 3.1

A>B

9.9 +/- 0.2

9.0 +/- 0.3

A>B

1.3 +/- 0.4

0.4 +/- 0.3

A>B

40.9 +/- 1.0

47.3 +/- 0.9

B>A

39.8 +/- 1.3

46.9 +/- 1.3

B>A

0.51 +/- 0.50

2.09 +/- 0.40

B>A

10.9 +/- 0.3

16.5 +/- 0.2

B>A

0.001 Number found

Odds = 5.41; z =

dead per pair

2.10; n = 106; P = 0.036

1st litter pup

F1, 96 = 19.77; P

weight (g)

<0.001

3rd litter pup

F1, 95 = 16.97; P

weight (g)

<0.001

1st litter malefemale weight difference (g) Ammonia (ppm)

F1, 96 = 6.02; P = 0.016 F1, 101 = 273.33; P <0.001

612 613

27

614

Figure 1

615

A

616

Percentage of pups cannibalised

6

*

5 4 3 2 1 0 3–4

14

Cage-cleaning interval (days)

617 618

7

B

Percentage of pairs that cannibalised pups

619 100

80

60

40

20

0 3–4

7

14

Cage-cleaning interval (days) 620 621 622

28

623

Figure 2

624 625

Probability of cannibalism in a litter

0.3 Fortnightly Once-weekly Twice-weekly

0.25 0.2 0.15 0.1 0.05 0 0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

Age at first cage-cleaning (days) 626 627 628 629 630 631 632

29

633

Figure 3

634 635 636

A

B *

120

120 100

Total pups weaned

Total pups born

100 80 60 40 20

60 40 20

0

0 A

B

A

Building

637 638

80

B

Building

C

Number of pups per litter

12

* Twice-weekly Once-Weekly Fortnightly

10 8 6 4 2 0 A

639

B

Building

640

30

641

Figure 4

642 643

A

B 90 80

120

Number of litters born

Number of litters born

140

100 80 60 40 20

70 60 50 40 30 20 10

0

0 0

644 645 646 647

1

2

3

0

Days since last cleaning

1

2

3

4

5

6

Days since last cleaning

C

Number of litters born

60 50 40 30 20 10 0 0

648 649

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

Days since last cleaning

31

Effects of cage-cleaning frequency on laboratory rat ...

Cannibalism was rare, affecting about 2.6% of all the pups born, which is approximately consistent with previous studies (Chantry, 1964; Reynolds, 1981;. DeSantis & Schmaltz, 1984), so cleaning frequency had no significant effect on pup mortality generally. Nevertheless, under these clean conditions and with this outbred.

201KB Sizes 1 Downloads 211 Views

Recommend Documents

On letter frequency effects
Aug 19, 2011 - be stronger for token frequency counts compared with both type frequency and frequency rank, stronger for frequency .... Connectionist models of letter and word processing. (such as the ... Stimuli and design ..... Database.

On the Effects of Frequency Scaling Over Capacity ... - Springer Link
Jan 17, 2013 - Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013 .... the scaling obtained by MH in wireless radio networks) without scaling the carrier ...

On the Effects of Frequency Scaling Over Capacity ... - Springer Link
Nov 7, 2012 - Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Northeastern ... In underwater acoustic communication systems, both bandwidth and received signal ... underwater acoustic channels, while network coding showed better performance than M

Effects of degree-frequency correlations on network ...
Jan 28, 2013 - recent years researchers have started to explore the effect of correlations .... 2: (Colour on-line) Illustration of phase-locking for sub- linear, linear, and .... and k0 =50 (all SF networks we use in this letter were generated using

Effects of Microwaves and Radio Frequency Energy on the Central ...
THE NON-THERMAL CAMP AND THUS DESERVING OF CENSURE. ..... THESE HYPOTHESES MUST THEN BE SUBJECTED TO EXPERIMEt4TAL TEST.

Effects of degree-frequency correlations on network ...
Jan 28, 2013 - ... of Physics and Astronomy, Northwestern University - Evanston, IL 60208, USA ... gain insight into the mechanism behind synchronization,.

effects of microwaves and radio frequency energy on the central ...
AND ttATHEMATICAL MODELING OF. EFFECTS. MUCH OF THE DATA IS AVAILABLE IN VARIOUS JOURNALS. THERE IS A SUBSTANTIAL AMOUNT THOUGH THAT I HAVE NOT HAD TIME TO. PREPARE FOR PUBLICATION. THIS LATER I SHALL EMPHASIZE. MY INTEREST IN THIS AREA BEGAN WHEN I

Effects of ethanol on intracorporeal structures of the rat - Springer Link
... School of medicine, Karaelmas University, Kozlu-. 67600, Zonguldak, Turkey. Phone: +90-372-2610169; Fax: +90-372-2610155. E-mail: [email protected].

Effects of growth hormone on insulin release in the rat
and tests performed 2 h after recovery. In 3 rats ... perifusion solutions were kept at 37 prior to circulation through ..... Since secretory data were expressed on.

Acute effects of fatty acids on insulin secretion from rat ...
1967). Since the beta-cell contains little glycogen. (Ashcroft et al. 1972) the main fuel store is likely to be triacylglycerol. However, rates of de novo fatty acid syn- thesis in islets are low (Berne 1975a); the fatty acid moieties of the triacylg

AD 698 195 EFFECTS OF MICROWAVES AND RADIO FREQUENCY ...
"PROVED" THAT NERVES CAN NOT BE AFFECTED BY RF ENERGY. ... WERE MARGINAL STUDIES ON STIMULATION OF PERIPHERAL NERVE ENDINGS.