Dynamic
Communication
and
Connectivity
in
Frontal
Networks
 
 Bradley
Voytek1
and
Robert
T.
Knight1,2
 
 1Helen
Wills
Neuroscience
Institute
and
2Department
of
Psychology


University
of
California,
Berkeley,
CA
94720
 
 
 How
do
we
maintain
a
stable
percept
of
the
world
in
the
face
of
the
powerful
drive
 of
neuroplasticity
in
both
health
and
disease?
This
dichotomy
forms
one
of
the
most
 fundamental
unanswered
questions
in
neuroscience
concerning
the
balance
 between
the
dynamic,
plastic
underpinnings
of
our
neurobiology
and
the
relative
 stability
of
our
cognition.
The
brain
undergoes
massive
changes
in
size,
morphology,
 and
connectivity
during
normal
development
(see
Fig.
1,
Gogtay
et
al.,
2004)
and
 aging
(Sowell
et
al.,
2003)
as
well
as
in
response
to
brain
injury
(Alsott
et
al.,
2009;
 Carmichael,
2003),
yet
we
can
maintain
a
relatively
stable
sense
of
cognition
and
 self
during
the
lifespan.

Human
brains,
each
with
over
100
billion
neurons,
develop
 similarly
despite
the
wide
variations
in
environment
and
experience.

However,
 within
the
bounds
of
this
stability
there
exists
a
wide
range
of
variability
and
 capacity
for
change.
Here
we
will
discuss
the
role
of
neuroplasticity
in
frontal
lobe‐ dependent
cognition
by
examining
the
localization
of
attention
and
memory
 functions
in
the
brain
and
how
these
seemingly
fixed
locations
may
reflect
flexible
 neural
networks
that
change
communication
properties
as
required
by
behavior.
 



 Figure
1.
 Changes
in
grey
matter
volume
with
normal
development

(Adapted
from
Gogtay
et
 al.,
2004).
This
figure
illustrates
the
structural
plasticity
of
the
neocortex
in
the
 developing
human
brain,
especially
in
association
cortex,
during
childhood.
Note
the
 relative
stability
of
primary
sensorimotor
and
visual
areas
by
puberty
in
contrast
to
 the
plasticity
of
the
childhood
frontal
and
temporal
association
cortices.
 
 
 Localization
of
Cognitive
Functions
 Localization
of
cognitive
functions
in
the
human
brain
poses
a
major
problem
in
 modern
neuroscience
(Brett,
Johnsrude,
&
Owen,
2002;
Young,
Hilgetag,
&
Scannell,
 2000).
First,
there
is
the
problem
of
comparing
localization
of
function
data
across
 methodologies
and
across
subjects
and
rectifying
findings
from
various
 neuroimaging
and
neuropsychological
methodologies—each
with
their
own
 limitations
and
underlying
assumptions—with
computational,
lesion,
and
animal
 studies.
This
presents
a
daunting
prospect
for
any
investigator.
Second,
 neuroscientists
face
the
inherent
morphological
variability
across
subjects;
 currently,
any
claims
to
cortical
functional
specificity
are
probabilistic
claims
in


that—barring
direct
cortical
stimulation
mapping—one
cannot
guarantee
that
a
 specific
cortical
region
plays
a
specific
functional
role.
For
example,
direct
cortical
 stimulation
mapping
suggests
frontal,
temporal,
and
parietal
sites
are
all
involved
in
 language
functions,
yet
the
specific
neuroanatomy
of
these
sites
differs
widely
 across
subjects
(Sanai,
Mirzadeh,
&
Berger,
2008).
 These
problems
are
not
just
theoretical
or
didactic
issues:
neurosurgeons
 performing
surgical
tissue
resections
must
use
intraoperative
cortical
stimulation
 mapping
to
ensure
that
the
cortical
tissue
to
be
removed
is
not
“eloquent”
(language
 or
motor)
cortex.
Such
stimulations
are
performed
while
the
patient
is
awake
and
 performing
cognitive
and
behavioral
tasks.
During
this
testing
period
the
surgeon
 electrically
stimulates
different
brain
regions
to
monitor
speech
or
motor
arrest.
 This
method—although
decades
old—is
still
widely
employed
because
of
the
known
 variability
in
functional
localization
and
cortical
morphology
across
subjects.
 Although
the
functional
localization
story
appears
bleak
at
the
level
of
a
 single
individual,
cerebral
regions
of
functional
localization
are
clearly
observed
 when
averaged
across
a
group
of
subjects
with
neuroimaging
techniques
such
as
 functional
magnetic
resonance
imaging
(fMRI)
and
positron
emission
tomography
 (PET).
Most
studies
rely
upon
the
principle
of
cognitive
subtraction,
originally
 established
in
reaction
time
studies
by
Franciscus
Donders
(Donders,
1869).
The
 underlying
assumption
in
these
studies
is
that
activity
in
brain
networks
alters
in
a
 task‐dependent
manner
that
becomes
evident
after
averaging
many
event‐related
 responses
and
comparing
those
against
a
baseline
condition.
Deviations
from
this


baseline
reflect
a
change
in
the
neuronal
processing
demands
required
to
perform
 the
task
of
interest.
 Although
both
the
cognitive
subtraction
method
(Friston
et
al.,
1996)
and
 assumptions
regarding
baseline
activity
(Gusnard
&
Raichle,
2001)
have
their
own
 problems,
these
methods
provide
details
of
functional
localization
that
can
then
be
 tested
and
corroborated
using
other
methodologies,
including
lesions
studies.
The
 interpretation
of
these
localization
results
is
confounded,
however,
by
a
lack
of
 clarity
in
what
is
meant
for
a
“function”
to
be
localized.
For
example,
Young
and
 colleagues
(2000)
noted
that
for
a
given
function
to
be
localizable
that
function
 “must
be
capable
of
being
considered
both
structurally
and
functionally
discrete”;
a
 property
that
the
brain
is
incapable
of
assuming
due
to
the
intricate,
large‐scale
 neuronal
interconnectivity.

 Thus,
discussing
behavioral
functions
outside
of
the
context
of
the
larger
 cortical
and
subcortical
networks
involved
with
that
function
is
a
poorly
posed
 problem.
Therefore,
the
scientific
study
of
cognition
requires
detailed
 neuroanatomical
and
connectivity
information
to
compliment
functional
activity
 findings.

The
current
effort
to
map
a
human
connectome
(Sporns,
Tononi,
&
Kötter,
 2005)
will
provide
researchers
with
the
neuroanatomical
roadmap
necessary
to
 examine
changes
in
large‐scale
cortical
network
activity
during
cognition.
 
 The
Lesion
Method
 While
functional
neuroimaging
techniques
such
as
fMRI
and
PET
have
advanced
our
 understanding
of
regional
specificity,
the
lesion
method
provides
the
strongest
case


in
the
argument
for
causality
in
functional
neuroanatomy;
i.e.,
brain
region
A
can
be
 assumed
to
play
an
important
role
in
the
network
supporting
function
X
if
a
lesion
 to
A
impairs
function
X.
Research
on
humans
with
focal
brain
lesions
(e.g.,
Fig.
2)
 has
provided
seminal
information
with
regards
to
our
understanding
of
which
brain
 regions
contribute
to
specific
behavioral,
sensory,
and
cognitive
functions
(Rorden
 &
Karnath,
2004).
For
example,
because
prefrontal
(PFC)
and
basal
ganglia
lesions
 lead
to
working
memory
deficits
(Voytek
&
Knight,
submitted;
Müller
&
Knight
 2006;
Tsuchida
&
Fellows,
2009),
the
PFC
can
be
said
to
play
an
important,
if
not
 necessary
role
in
working
memory
networks.
 



 Figure
2.
 Patient
lesion
reconstructions.
These
structural
MRI
slices
illustrate
the
lesion
 overlap
across
six
patients
with
unilateral
PFC
lesions.
All
lesions
are
normalized
to
 the
left
hemisphere
for
comparison
although
two
patients
had
right
hemisphere
 lesions
(Adapted
from
Voytek
et
al.,
submitted).
Examining
groups
of
patients
with
 stereotyped
lesions
allows
researchers
to
test
the
role
of
specific
regions
in
 behavior.
Software
reconstructions
were
performed
using
MRIcro
(Rorden
&
Brett,
 2000).
 
 By
combining
lesion
studies
with
neuroimaging
techniques,
researchers
can
 identify
other
brain
regions
associated
with
a
certain
behavior.
For
example,
 research
using
scalp
electroencephalography
(EEG)
has
shown
that
unilateral
PFC
 lesions
cause
lateralized
deficits
in
top‐down
modulation
of
activity
in
visual
 extrastriate
cortex
during
attention
(Fig.
3,
Barceló,
Suwazano,
&
Knight,
2000;
Yago


et
al.,
2004)
and
working
memory
(Voytek
&
Knight,
submitted),
which
makes
EEG
a
 powerful
tool
for
investigating
the
network
dynamics
subserving
cognition.
 



 Figure
3.
 Examining
the
effects
of
unilateral
PFC
lesions
on
attention
networks.
A,
Illustration
 of
the
lateralization
of
early
visual
activity
modulated
by
attention.
For
healthy
 control
subjects
(top),
lateralized,
attended
stimuli
lead
to
early
(~150ms)
activity
 increases
in
visual
extrastriate
cortex
(orange
region).
For
patients
with
unilateral
 PFC
lesions
(shaded
region,
bottom),
normal
attention‐related
activity
increases
are
 seen
for
stimuli
presented
ipsilesionally
(orange),
however
when
stimuli
are
 presented
contralesionally
patients
show
activity
deficits
compared
to
controls
 (blue).
This
effect
is
seen
in
scalp
EEG
in
B
(Adapted
from
Barceló,
Suwazano,
&
 Knight,
2000).
 
 While
the
underlying
notion
of
brain
damage
disrupting
function
is
fairly
 obvious—damaging
parts
of
a
machine
prevent
the
machine
from
working
 optimally—the
specific
effects
of
brain
damage
are
neither
obvious
nor
always
 predictable.
There
are
several
factors
that
prohibit
accurate
prediction
of
which
 deficits
will
manifest
after
a
given
brain
lesion.
This
is
largely
due
to
the
fact
that
we


are
still
uncertain
with
regards
to
the
accuracy
of
regional
localization
of
function
 and
the
poorly
posed
nature
of
the
functional
localization
question
in
general.
 Because
the
probability
distribution
of
functional
localization
across
subjects
is
 broad,
especially
across
cortical
association
areas
(Sanai,
Mirzadeh,
&
Berger,
2008),
 the
importance
of
distributed
cortical
networks
in
behavior
and
subsequent
 recovery
cannot
be
ignored.
 Nevertheless,
working
with
patients
with
circumscribed
frontal
brain
lesions
 provides
us
with
insight
into
how
frontal
cortex
interacts
with
the
rest
of
the
brain
 to
give
rise
to
cognitive
functions.
When
combined
with
computational
and
 behavioral
methodology
and/or
neuroimaging,
the
lesion
method
allows
 researchers
to
examine
exactly
which
areas
are
critical
for
which
cognitive
 functions.
For
example,
recent
work
by
Badre
and
colleagues
took
advantage
of
the
 inherent
differences
in
lesion
size
and
extent
in
their
patient
populations
to
examine
 the
rostral/caudal
organization
of
cognitive
and
action
control
in
the
frontal
cortex
 (Badre
et
al.,
2009).
While
this
“messiness”
of
lesion
size,
extent,
and
location
has
 traditionally
been
viewed
as
a
major
drawback
of
the
lesion
method,
it
is
the
 cornerstone
of
voxel‐based
lesion‐symptom
mapping
(VLSM)
(Fig.
4,
Bates
et
al.,
 2003).
This
method
requires
a
detailed
neuroanatomical
scan
of
every
patient;
t‐ tests
are
then
performed
at
every
voxel
on
a
variable
of
interest
(e.g.,
a
cognitive
 task)
where
the
statistical
“groups”
are
defined
by
whether
the
patient
has
a
lesion
 in
that
specific
voxel
or
not.
This
clever
technique
allows
researchers
to
map
voxel‐ by‐voxel
which
regions
are
most
important
for
a
cognitive
function.



 Figure
4.
 Example
of
VLSM
(Adapted
from
Bates
et
al.,
2003).
These
maps
show
speech
 fluency
(A­C)
and
language
comprehension
(D­F)
in
101
aphasic
stroke
patients.
 Color
represents
the
effect
of
lesion
on
behavior
with
large
t‐values
suggesting
a
 significant
relationship
between
the
presence
of
a
lesion
and
a
behavioral
deficit.
 
 Recent
work
has
expanded
the
lesion
method
into
computational
modeling.
 Using
a
cortically‐plausible
network
architecture
researchers
have
shown
the
 effects
of
lesions
on
functional
connectivity
(Alstott
et
al.,
2009;
Young,
Hilgetag,
&
 Scannell,
2000)
and
on
oscillatory
dynamics
(Honey
&
Sporns,
2008)
demonstrating
 activity
changes
in
remote
brain
areas
(Reggia,
2004)
not
directly
connected
to
the
 lesioned
brain
region
(Young,
Hilgetag,
&
Scannell,
2000).
These
findings
suggest
 that
lesions
to
highly
connected
critical
hubs—including
frontal
and
parietal
 regions—result
in
widespread
changes
in
functional
connectivity
and
oscillatory
 communication.
 
 Recovery
and
Compensation


Predicting
the
course
of
recovery
from
brain
damage
is
confounded
by
a
lack
of
 understanding
about
the
extent
and
time
course
of
recovery
possible
across
 different
regions
of
the
central
nervous
system.
Neural
plasticity
is
critical
for
 functional
recovery
after
brain
damage
with
improvement
possible
even
20
years
 after
the
initial
injury
(Bach‐y‐Rita,
1990).
There
are
several
theories
of
recovery
of
 function
(Grafman,
2000),
including:
cortical
compensation
by
perilesion
and
intact
 homologous
brain
regions
(Wundt,
1902)
or
subcortical
(Van
Vleet
et
al.,
2003)
 structures;
diaschisis
reversal
(von
Monakow,
1969);
unmasking
(Lytton,
Williams,
 &
Sober,
1999);
distributed
cortical
representations
(Jackson,
1958);
and
axonal
 sprouting
and
neurogenesis
(Carmichael
et
al.,
2001).
Many
of
these
theories
 predate
neuroimaging
and
were
based
on
clinical
observations
of
patients
with
 brain
damage.
In
1902,
Wilhelm
Wundt
noted
that,

 
 …in
both
simple
and
complex
disturbances,
there
is
usually
a
gradual
 restoration
of
the
functions
in
the
course
of
time.
This
is
probably
 effected
by
the
vicarious
functioning
of
some,
generally
a
neighboring
 cortical
region
in
place
of
that
which
is
disturbed
(in
disturbances
of
 speech,
perhaps
it
is
the
opposite,
before
untrained,
side
that
comes
into
 play).
 
 This
latter
point
was
proved
in
a
recent
paper
wherein
Blasi,
et
al.
demonstrated
 that
patients
who
have
recovered
from
Broca’s
aphasia
due
to
left
frontal
stroke


show
fMRI
activation
in
the
right
frontal
Broca’s
area
homologue
(Fig.
5A,
Blasi
et
 al.,
2002).
 The
fact
that
the
brain
is
not
a
static
machine,
but
rather
a
fluctuating
 (plastic),
self‐repairing
organ
(Cramer,
2008),
provides
an
important
confound
to
 lesion‐based
research.
For
example,
most
lesion
studies
that
demonstrate
 behavioral
deficits
in
humans
are
performed
on
patients
who
have
had
sudden
 (acute)
brain
damage
(e.g.,
stroke
or
trauma)
precisely
because
these
patients
show
 the
strongest
behavioral
deficits.
In
contrast,
patients
who
have
undergone
surgical
 resections
to
remove
cancerous
cerebral
tissue
tend
to
show
fewer
deficits
before
 and
after
their
surgeries
(Desmurget,
Bonnetblanc,
&
Duffau,
2006)
compared
to
a
 patient
with
a
comparably
size
lesion
from
a
stroke.
This
phenomenon
is
 interpreted
as
recovery
processes
resulting
from
compensation
by
other
brain
 regions
in
cases
of
slow‐growing
lesions.
Because
the
lesions
are
slow‐growing
 rather
than
rapidly‐occurring
(such
as
from
stroke),
the
hypothesis
is
that
the
 deficits
resulting
from
the
lesion
are
minimized
due
to
the
incrementally
slow
rate
 of
growth
permitting
compensatory
processes
to
mask
those
deficits.
By
definition,
 acute
lesions,
on
the
other
hand,
result
in
rapid
tissue
damage
that
cannot
be
 (immediately)
compensated
for.
Thus,
though
patient
work
is
invaluable,
the
 temporality
of
the
lesion
(both
onset
time
and
time
since
damage)
should
not
be
 discounted.

 Given
the
number
of
brain
regions
needed
to
support
cognitive
functions,
it
is
 not
unreasonable,
given
the
variety
of
recovery
theories,
to
hypothesize
that
 cognitive
recovery
could
be
supported
by
any
part
of
the
cognitive
network.
The


PFC
however
plays
an
important
role
in
cognitive
networks
by
biasing
information
 flow
in
other
regions
to
favor
positive
behavioral
outcomes
(Miller
&
Cohen,
2001).

 Therefore,
the
PFC
may
play
a
privileged
role
in
cognitive
compensation.
For
 example,
although
patients
with
lateral
PFC
lesions
have
lasting
attention
and
 working
memory
deficits
(e.g.,
Voytek
&
Knight,
submitted;
Barceló,
Suwazano,
&
 Knight,
2000),
cognitive
functions
can
recover
somewhat
over
time
(Voytek
et
al.,
 submitted).
Numerous
studies
suggest
that
the
PFC
plays
a
diverse
role
in
a
wide
 range
of
cognitive
functions
involved
in
the
allocation
and
control
of
visual
attention
 and
working
memory.
One
hypothesis
is
that
the
PFC
maintains
an
association
 between
endogenous
elements
in
working
memory
while
an
unknown
neuronal
 mechanism
compares
these
endogenous
representations
to
exogenous
visual
 information
as
it
is
processed
in
extrastriate
visual
areas
(Barceló,
Suwazano,
&
 Knight,
2000;
Kimberg
&
Farah,
1993).

 It
is
important
to
note
that
neuropsychological
testing
alone
can
be
 misleading
concerning
the
extent
of
recovery
after
PFC
damage.
For
example,
if,
 during
an
attention
task,
visual
stimuli
are
presented
full‐field,
that
is,
presented
in
 the
center
of
the
visual
field
and
with
unrestrained
eye‐movements,
patients
with
 unilateral
PFC
lesions
do
not
show
obvious
visual
attention
deficits.
However,
if
 visual
stimuli
are
lateralized
to
the
left
or
right
visual
hemifield
by
a
matter
of
a
few
 degrees
and
central
fixation
is
maintained,
then
deficits
in
visual
working
memory
 (Voytek
&
Knight,
submitted)
and
attention
(Barceló,
Suwazano,
&
Knight,
2000)
are
 clearly
evident.



Visual
stimulus
lateralization
takes
advantage
of
the
neuroanatomy
of
the
 mammalian
visual
system
such
that
stimuli
presented
to
the
right
visual
hemifield
 preferentially
activate
the
left
visual
cortex
(and
vice
versa)
before
that
information
 is
then
transferred
to
the
opposite
visual
cortex
via
the
corpus
callosum.
Such
 lateralized
designs
increase
statistical
power
in
that
patients
can
serve
partially
as
 their
own
controls
(i.e.,
“good”
hemifield
vs.
“bad”
hemifield;
see
Fig.
3A),
thus
 allowing
for
a
within‐subjects
comparison
of
the
effects
of
the
brain
lesion
on
a
 cognitive
function
for
contralesionally‐
versus
ipsilesionally‐presented
stimuli.
 Nevertheless,
even
in
lateralized
visual
attention
and
working
memory
 paradigms
patients
with
unilateral
PFC
damage—though
worse
than
control
 subjects
when
stimuli
are
presented
contralesionally—still
perform
well
above
 chance
levels.
This
finding
is
somewhat
in
contrast
to
what
is
observed
in
lesion
and
 neuroimaging
studies
of
primary
cortical
functions.
Neuroimaging
studies
of
 movement
or
visual
processing
localize
these
processes
to
motor
and
visual
cortex,
 respectively.
Lesions
to
primary
motor
or
primary
visual
cortex
lead
to
striking
and
 permanent
deficits
(hemiparesis
or
cortical
blindness,
in
these
specific
cases).
 Conversely,
while
functional
neuroimaging
studies
show
task‐dependent
PFC
 activation
during
attentional
control
and
working
memory,
lesions
to
the
PFC
lead
 to
an
incomplete
loss
of
those
functions.
This
discrepancy
may
have
any
number
of
 underlying
causes,
including
any
combination
of
the
following:
1.
Research
 paradigms
used
to
assess
cognitive
deficits
may
be
less
sensitive
and
less
specific
 than
those
used
to
examine
motor
or
sensory
deficits;
2.
Cognitive
processes
 dependent
on
association
cortex
may
be
more
widely
distributed
across
a
broader


network
than
those
dependent
on
primary
cortex,
making
cognitive
processes
more
 resilient
to
a
single
focal
lesion;
3.
Compensatory
mechanisms
may
be
facilitating
 damaged
cognitive
functions
more
than
primary
functions.
 In
order
for
neuronal
activity
differences
to
be
considered
“compensatory”,
 Davis
et
al.
(2008)
have
outlined
at
least
two
criteria
that
must
be
met.
First,
novel
 activity
increases
not
seen
in
normal
controls
(but
seen
in
e.g.,
lesion
patients)
must
 be
associated
with
correct
behavioral
outcomes.
Second,
deficits
in
processing
by
 one
region
must
be
associated
with
increases
in
activity
in
the
putative
 compensatory
region.
These
criteria
are
important
because
activity
increases
 interpreted
as
“compensatory”
may
in
fact
more
simply
reflect
a
global
increase
in
 cortical
activity
due
to
increases
in
difficulty
in
performing
a
task
for
lesion
patients
 compared
to
control
subjects
(Hillary
et
al.,
2006).
That
is,
because
of
the
lesion,
 more
cognitive
resources
are
recruited
in
order
to
correctly
perform
the
task
 compared
to
controls.
 In
the
context
of
unilateral
PFC
damage
and
its
effects
on
attention
and
 working
memory,
Voytek
et
al.
(submitted)
hypothesized
that
the
intact,
undamaged
 PFC
compensates
for
the
damaged
cortex
in
a
load‐dependent
manner
as
required
 by
task
demands.
What
was
observed
(Fig.
5B),
consistent
with
the
first
criterion
 for
compensation,
was
that
increases
in
activity
over
the
intact
PFC
are
enhanced
on
 correct
trials
when
the
damaged
PFC
is
challenged
with
lateralized
visual
working
 memory
or
attention
demands.
With
regards
to
the
second
criterion,
their
 experimental
designs
preferentially
challenged
the
damaged
hemisphere
in
patients
 with
unilateral
PFC
damage,
and
increases
in
activity
over
the
intact
PFC
were
seen


in
conjunction
with
top‐down
deficits
in
the
visual
extrastriate
cortex
of
the
 damaged
hemisphere.
It
is
important
to
highlight
that
the
decreased
posterior
 extrastriate
responses
seen
in
cognitive
experiments
with
patients
with
unilateral
 PFC
damage
(Voytek
&
Knight,
submitted;
Barceló,
Suwazano,
&
Knight,
2000)
are
 only
seen
when
stimuli
are
presented
to
the
contralesional
hemifield.
If
we
are
to
 assume
that
these
posterior
responses
normally
index
behavior
and
performance— and
PFC
patients
show
attenuated
extrastriate
responses
even
when
correctly
 performing
the
task—then
logically
there
must
be
some
other
brain
regions
 compensating
for
the
lesioned
cortex.



 Figure
5.
 Examples
of
compensatory
activity
after
frontal
damage.
A,
Compared
to
healthy
 control
subjects,
patients
with
damage
to
left
inferior
frontal
gyrus
who
have
 recovered
from
speech
deficits
show
increased
activation
in
the
homologous
area
in
 the
intact
hemisphere
(blue
arrow)
and
decreased
activation
in
the
damaged
region
 (red
arrows;
Adapted
from
Blasi
et
al.,
2002).
B,
Using
lateralized
visual
stimulus
 designs,
Voytek
et
al.,
(submitted)
showed
that
patients
with
unilateral
PFC
lesions


(shaded
regions)
show
increased
activity
over
the
intact
PFC
only
when
the
 damaged
hemisphere
was
directly
challenged
with
visual
stimuli.
This
activity
was
 not
seen
in
control
subjects
and
it
scaled
with
cognitive
demands.
 
 As
previously
stated,
research
indicates
that
the
perilesion
cortex
and
the
 homologous
intact
contralateral
cortex
may
both
be
involved
in
recovery
and
that
 there
is
long‐range,
intracortical
reorganization
of
behaviorally‐
and
recovery‐ relevant
pathways
(Nudo,
2007;
Dancause,
2006).
Thus,
Voytek
et
al.
proposed
that
 the
visual
information
delivered
to
the
contralesional
hemisphere
is
transferred
 trans‐callosally
to
the
intact
hemisphere
where
the
intact
PFC
then
assumes
task
 control
as
needed
on
a
trial‐by‐trial
basis.
Support
for
this
contention
is
provided
by
 studies
in
non‐human
primates
revealing
that
top‐down
PFC
control
over
visual
 cortex
during
memory
retrieval
relies
on
callosal
information
transfer
(Hasegawa
et
 al.,
1998;
Tomita
et
al.,
1999).
Thus,
if
transcallosal
information
transfer
could
be
 blocked,
then
behavioral
deficits
should
be
enhanced.
 As
discussed
previously,
in
contrast
with
cognitive
deficits,
primary
motor
 and
sensory
functions
rarely
recover
in
adults
who
suffer
cortical
damage,
although
 other
modalities
may
take
over
intact
sensory
cortex
deprived
of
input
due
to
 peripheral
damage
(Sadato
et
al.,
1996).
Unlike
adults
with
primary
cortical
damage,
 children
who
have
had
a
surgical
hemispherectomy,
for
example,
can
regain
motor
 control
of
the
affected
limbs
(Benecke
1991);
such
recovery
can
be
seen
even
in
 children
with
massive
and
severe
cortical
damage
(e.g.,
Distelmaier
et
al.,
2007).
In
 contrast,
others
have
observed
a
surprising
normality
among
patients
missing
 massive
amounts
of
their
cortical
tissue
(Lewin,
1980).
While
deficits
caused
by
 lesions
to
PFC
are
more
likely
to
recover
if
lesions
occur
later
in
life—and
this


recovery
may
be
dependant
upon
having
some
amount
of
intact
PFC
(Kolb
&
Gibb,
 1990)—
children
with
PFC
damage
may
have
lasting
cognitive
impairment
(Kolb
&
 Gibb,
1990).
The
interaction
between
age
and
location
of
lesion
with
behavioral
 recovery
may
reflect
a
deeper
relationship
with
the
evolution
of
cognitive
and
 sensory
functions
in
primates
(Anderson,
2007)
wherein
cognitive
functions,
having
 evolved
more
recently,
are
more
distributed
across
cortex
and
thus
more
resistant
 to
focal
brain
damage
once
those
functions
have
developed
in
adulthood.
 Integrating
all
of
the
prior
points
it
may
be
that
the
farther
away
from
 primary
cortical
areas
a
region
is,
the
less
predictable
the
function
becomes.
This
 phenomenon
may
help
explain
why
we
have
fairly
robust
sensory
and
motor
 homunculi
in
the
primary
(“lower”)
cortical
areas,
but
no
reliable
mapping
in
the
 “higher”
sensory
and
motor
association
cortices.
This
is
illustrated
by
example
from
 clinical
observations:
a
patient
with
damage
to
the
premotor
cortex
is
more
likely
to
 recover
motor
functions
than
a
patient
with
a
lesion
of
primary
motor
cortex,
who
 in
turn
is
more
likely
to
naturally
recover
than
a
person
with
a
lower
motor
neuron
 lesion
in
the
spinal
cord.
A
network
theoretic
view
of
this
phenomenon
would
 suggest
that
differences
between
the
focal
networks
of
primary
regions
and
 distributed
networks
of
the
functions
subserved
by
association
cortex
may
account
 for
these
differences
in
recovery.
Given
the
above
caveats,
in
order
to
study
human
 cortical
recovery
of
function
one
must
carefully
balance
recovery
likelihood
with
 probability
of
functional
localization.
That
is,
in
theory,
one
is
more
likely
to
find
a
 reliable
deficit
across
subjects
with
damage
to
primary
cortical
regions,
but
less
 likely
to
observe
recovery
in
these
patients.


References
 Alstott,
 J.,
 Breakspear,
 M.,
 Hagmann,
 P.,
 Cammoun,
 L.,
 Sporns,
 O.,
 &
 Friston,
 K.
 (2009).
Modeling
the
impact
of
lesions
in
the
human
brain.
PLoS
Comput.
Biol.
5,
1‐ 12.
 
 Anderson,
 M.
 (2007)
 Evolution
 of
 Cognitive
 Function
 via
 Redeployment
 of
 Brain
 Areas.
Neuroscientist
13,
13‐21.
 
 Bach‐y‐Rita,
P.
(1990).
Brain
plasticity
as
a
basis
for
recovery
of
function
in
humans.
 Neuropsychologia
28,
547‐554.
 
 Badre,
 D.,
 Hoffman,
 J.,
 Cooney,
 J.,
 &
 D’Esposito,
 M.
 (2009).
 Hierarchical
 cognitive
 control
deficits
following
damage
to
the
human
frontal
lobe.
Nat.
Neurosci.
12,
515‐ 522.
 
 Barceló,
 F.,
 Suwazono,
 S.,
 &
 Knight,
 R.T.
 (2000).
 Prefrontal
 modulation
 of
 visual
 processing
in
humans.
Nat.
Neurosci.
3,
399‐403.
 
 Bates,
 E.,
 Wilson,
 S.,
 Saygin,
 A.,
 Dick,
 F.,
 Sereno,
 M.,
 Knight,
 R.T.,
 &
 Dronkers,
 N.
 (2003).
Voxel‐based
lesion–symptom
mapping.
Nat.
Neurosci.
6,
448‐450.
 
 Benecke,
R.,
Meyer,
B.U.,
&
Freund,
H.J.
(1991).
Reorganisation
of
descending
motor
 pathways
 in
 patients
 after
 hemispherectomy
 and
 severe
 hemispheric
 lesions
 demonstrated
by
magnetic
brain
stimulation.
Exp.
Brain
Res.
83,
419‐426.
 
 Blasi,
 V.,
 Young,
 A.C.,
 Tansy,
 A.P.,
 Petersen,
 S.E.,
 Snyder,
 A.,
 &
 Corbetta,
 M.
 (2002).
 Word
 retrieval
 learning
 modulates
 right
 frontal
 cortex
 in
 patients
 with
 left
 frontal
 damage.
Neuron
36,
159‐170.
 
 Brett,
M.,
Johnsrude,
I.,
&
Owen,
A.
(2002).
The
problem
of
functional
localization
in
 the
human
brain.
Nat.
Rev.
Neurosci.
3,
243‐249.
 
 Carmichael,
 S.T.,
 Wei,
 L.,
 Rovainen,
 C.M.,
 &
 Woolsey,
 T.A.
 (2001).
 New
 patterns
 of
 intracortical
projections
after
focal
cortical
stroke.
Neurobiol.
Dis.
8,
910‐922.
 
 Carmichael,
S.
(2003).
Plasticity
of
cortical
projections
after
stroke.
Neuroscientist
9,
 64‐75.
 
 Cramer,
 S.
 (2008).
 Repairing
 the
 human
 brain
 after
 stroke:
 I.
 Mechanisms
 of
 spontaneous
recovery.
Ann.
Neurol.
63,
272‐287.
 


Dancause,
N.
(2006).
Vicarious
Function
of
Remote
Cortex
following
Stroke:
Recent
 Evidence
from
Human
and
Animal
Studies.
Neuroscientist
12,
489‐499.
 
 Davis,
S.W.,
Dennis,
N.A.,
Daselaar,
S.M.,
Fleck,
M.S.,
&
Cabeza,
R.
(2008).
Que
PASA?
 The
posterior‐anterior
shift
in
aging.
Cereb.
Cortex
18,
1201‐1209.
 
 Desmurget,
 M.,
 Bonnetblanc,
 F.,
 &
 Duffau,
 H.
 (2007).
 Contrasting
 acute
 and
 slow‐ growing
lesions:
a
new
door
to
brain
plasticity.
Brain
130,
898‐914.
 
 Distelmaier,
F.,
Richter‐Werkle,
R.,
Schaper,
J.,
Messing‐Juenger,
M.,
Mayatepek,
E.,
&
 Rosenbaum,
 T.
 (2007).
 "How
 Much
 Brain
 Is
 Really
 Necessary?"
 A
 Case
 of
 Complex
 Cerebral
Malformation
and
Its
Clinical
Course.
J.
Child
Neurol.
22,
756‐760.
 
 Donders,
 F.C.
 (1868).
 Die
 Schnelligkeit
 psychischer
 Prozesse.
 Archiv
 für
 Anatomie
 und
Physiologie
und
wissenschaftliche
Medizin,
657‐681.
 
 Friston,
K.J.,
Price,
C.J.,
Fletcher,
P.,
Moore,
C.,
Frackowiak,
R.S.,
&
Dolan,
R.J.
(1996).
 The
trouble
with
cognitive
subtraction.
NeuroImage

4,
97‐104.
 
 Gogtay,
N.,
Giedd.,
J.N.,
Lusk.,
L.,
Hayashi,
K.M.,
Greenstein,
D.,
Vaituzis,
A.C.,
Nugent,
 T.F.,
 Herman,
 D.H.,
 Clasen,
 L.S.,
 Toga,
 A.,
 Rapoport,
 J.L.,
 &
 Thompson,
 P.
 (2004).
 Dynamic
 mapping
 of
 human
 cortical
 development
 during
 childhood
 through
 early
 adulthood.
Proc.
Natl.
Acad.
Sci.
USA
101,
8174‐8179.
 
 Grafman,
 J.
 (2000).
 Conceptualizing
 functional
 neuroplasticity.
 J.
 Commun.
 Disord.
 33,
345‐355.
 
 Gusnard,
 D.A.
 &
 Raichle,
 M.E.
 (2001).
 Searching
 for
 a
 baseline:
 functional
 imaging
 and
the
resting
human
brain.
Nat.
Rev.
Neurosci.
2,
685‐694.
 
 Hasegawa,
 I.,
 Fukushima,
 T.,
 Ihara,
 T.,
 &
 Miyashita,
 Y.
 (1998).
 Callosal
 window
 between
 prefrontal
 cortices:
 Cognitive
 interaction
 to
 retrieve
 long‐term
 memory.
 Science
281,
814‐818.
 
 Hillary,
 F.,
 Genova,
 H.,
 Chiaravalloti,
 N.,
 Rypma,
 B.,
 &
 DeLuca,
 J.
 (2006).
 Prefrontal
 modulation
 of
 working
 memory
 performance
 in
 brain
 injury
 and
 disease.
 Hum.
 Brain
Mapp.
27,
837‐847.
 
 Honey,
 C.
 &
 Sporns,
 O.
 (2008).
 Dynamical
 consequences
 of
 lesions
 in
 cortical
 networks.
Hum.
Brain
Mapp.
29,
802‐809.
 


Jackson,
 J.H.
 (1958).
 A
 study
 of
 convulsions.
 In:
 Taylor,
 J.,
 Ed.,
 Selected
 Writings
 of
 John
Hughlings
Jackson.
(Staples:
London).
 
 Kimberg,
 D.Y.
 &
 Farah,
 M.J.
 (1993).
 A
 unified
 account
 of
 cognitive
 impairments
 following
 frontal
 lobe
 damage:
 the
 role
 of
 working
 memory
 in
 complex,
 organized
 behavior.
J.
Exp.
Psychol.
Gen.
122,
411‐428.
 
 Kolb,
 B.
 &
 Gibb,
 R.
 (1990).
 Anatomical
 correlates
 of
 behavioural
 change
 after
 neonatal
prefrontal
lesions
in
rats.
Prog.
Brain
Res.
85,
241‐255.
 
 Lewin,
R.
(1980).
Is
your
brain
really
necessary?
Science
210,
1232‐1234.
 
 Lytton,
 W.W.,
 Williams,
 S.T.,
 &
 Sober,
 S.J.
 (1999).
 Unmasking
 unmasked:
 Neural
 dynamics
following
stroke.
Prog.
Brain
Res.
121,
203‐218.
 
 Miller,
E.K.
&
Cohen,
J.D.
(2001).
An
integrative
theory
of
prefrontal
cortex
function.
 Annu.
Rev.
Neurosci.
24,
167‐202.
 
 Müller,
N.G.
&
Knight
R.T.
(2006).
The
functional
neuroanatomy
of
working
memory:
 contributions
of
human
brain
lesion
studies.
Neuroscience
139,
51‐58.
 
 Nudo,
R.J.
(2007).
Postinfarct
cortical
plasticity
and
behavioral
recovery.
Stroke
38,
 840‐845.
 
 Reggia,
 J.
 (2004).
 Neurocomputational
 models
 of
 the
 remote
 effects
 of
 focal
 brain
 damage.
Med.
Eng.
Phys.
26,
711‐722.
 
 Rorden,
 C.
 &
 Karnath,
 H.O.
 (2004).
 Using
 human
 brain
 lesions
 to
 infer
 function:
 A
 relic
from
a
past
era
in
the
fMRI
age?
Nat.
Rev.
Neurosci.
5,
813‐819.
 
 Sadato,
N.,
Pascual‐Leone,
A.,
Grafman,
J.,
Ibañez,
V.,
Deiber,
M.P.,
Dold,
G.,
&
Hallett,
 M.
 (1996).
 Activation
 of
 the
 primary
 visual
 cortex
 by
 Braille
 reading
 in
 blind
 subjects.
Nature
380,
526‐528.
 
 Sanai,
 N.,
 Mirzadeh,
 Z.,
 &
 Berger,
 M.
 (2008).
 Functional
 outcome
 after
 language
 mapping
for
glioma
resection.
N.
Engl.
J.
Med.
358,
18‐27.
 
 Sowell,
E.R.,
Peterson,
B.S.,
Thompson,
P.,
Welcome,
S.E.,
Henkenius,
A.L.,
&
Toga,
A.
 (2003).
Mapping
cortical
change
across
the
human
life
span.
 Nat.
Neurosci.
6,
209‐ 215.
 


Sporns,
 O.,
 Tononi,
 G.,
 &
 Kötter,
 R.
 (2005).
 
 The
 human
 connectome:
 A
 structural
 description
of
the
human
brain.
PLoS
Comp.
Biol.
1,
e42.
 
 Tomita,
 H.,
 Ohbayashi,
 M.,
 Nakahara,
 K.,
 Hasegawa,
 I.,
 &
 Miyashita,
 Y.
 (1999).
 Top‐ down
signal
from
prefrontal
cortex
in
executive
control
of
memory
retrieval.
Nature
 401,
699‐703.
 
 Tsuchida,
A.
&
Fellows,
L.K.
(2009).
Lesion
evidence
that
two
distinct
regions
within
 prefrontal
 cortex
 are
 critical
 for
 n‐back
 performance
 in
 humans.
 J.
 Cogn.
 Neurosci.
 21,
2263‐2275.
 
 Van
Vleet,
T.M.,
Heldt,
S.A.,
Pyter,
B.,
Corwin,
J.V.,
&
Reep,
R.L.
(2003).
Effects
of
light
 deprivation
on
recovery
from
neglect
and
extinction
induced
by
unilateral
lesions
of
 the
medial
agranular
cortex
and
dorsocentral
striatum.
Behav.
Brain
Res.
138,
165‐ 178.
 
 von
Monakow
C.
(1969).
Die
lokalisation
im
grosshirn
und
der
abbau
der
funktion
 durch
kortikale
herde.
In:
Pribram,
K.
H.
Ed.,
Mood,
States
and
Mind.
(Penguin
Books:
 London).
 
 Voytek,
 B.
 &
 Knight,
 R.T.
 (submitted).
 Prefrontal
 cortex
 and
 basal
 ganglia
 contributions
to
visual
working
memory.
 
 Voytek,
 B.,
 Davis,
 M.,
 Yago,
 E.,
 Barceló,
 F.,
 Vogel,
 E.K.,
 &
 Knight,
 R.T.
 (submitted).
 Dynamic
Neuroplasticity
after
Human
Prefrontal
Cortex
Damage.
 
 Wundt,
W.
(1902).
Outlines
of
Psychology,
2nd
Ed.
(Engelmann:
Leipzig).
 
 Yago,
E.,
Duarte,
A.,
Wong,
T.,
Barceló,
F.,
&
Knight,
R.T.
(2004).
Temporal
kinetics
of
 prefrontal
modulation
of
the
extrastriate
cortex
during
visual
attention.
Cogn.
Affect.
 Behav.
Neurosci.
4,
609‐617.
 
 Young,
M.,
Hilgetag,
C.,
&
Scannell,
J.
(2000).
On
imputing
function
to
structure
from
 the
behavioural
effects
of
brain
lesions.
Philos.
Trans.
R.
Soc.
Lond.,
B,
Biol.
Sci.
355,
 147‐161.


Dynamic Communication and Connectivity in Frontal ...

provides us with insight into how frontal cortex interacts with the rest of the ... Predicting the course of recovery from brain damage is confounded by a lack of.

4MB Sizes 0 Downloads 187 Views

Recommend Documents

Dynamic Connectivity and Packet Propagation Delay in ...
are transferred from a source to a destination in a multihop fashion with ..... 153–170. [10] H. Kesten, Aspects of first passage percolation, Lecture Notes in Math.

Dynamic causal modelling of effective connectivity ...
Mar 16, 2013 - In this Director task, around 50% of the time ..... contrast) and showed weaker effects overall than the main effect. Hence, we conducted ..... (A) VOIs used in the DCM analyses and illustration of the fixed connectivity between ...

PDF Frontal Lobe Seizures and Epilepsies in Children ...
... epilepsy in childhood and adolescence held at the Milan State University in October ... adult epilepsy specialists, psychiatrists, neuropsychologists and other ...

Corridors and connectivity
George Street, Brisbane 4001, Australia; *Author for correspondence (e-mail: [email protected]). Received 22 ... a means for detecting gene flow, direct methods. (i.e. trapping) are ..... (Compliance no. ISO 11794, Veterinary Marketing.

Connectivity
When you're on the road, you can stay connected with a 3G connectivity package from. Verizon Wireless. Chromebook that have built-in 3G include up to 100MB ...

Connectivity
Free data available for 2 years from the time you first activate your 3G service. 2. A day pass offers unlimited data access for 24 hours from the time of data purchase. 3. Any purchase of additional data expires after 30 days from the date of data p

COMMITTEE CONNECTIVITY IN THE UNITED ... - Semantic Scholar
random graph theory, statistics and computer science, we discuss what con- .... At the start of each two-year term, Representatives deliver written requests to ..... We call this Model A. Figure 4 shows that these fixed degree assignments ...

Dynamic Demand and Dynamic Supply in a Storable ...
In many markets, demand and/or supply dynamics are important and both firms and consumers are forward-looking. ... 1Alternative techniques for estimating dynamic games have been proposed by (Pesendorfer and Schmidt-. 3 ... Our estimation technique us

VWFA and its Connectivity in the Congenitally Deaf
Mar 18, 2014 - VWFA and a fronto-parietal network was similar to that of hearing ...... Y.B.), Major Project of National Social Science Foundation .... adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 100:10049–10054. Emmorey K, Weisberg J, McCullough S, ...

Digital connectivity in the Bay of Bengal region and beyond.pdf ...
Amsterdam. Milan. Oslo. Frankfurt. Chicago. Dallas. Washington. London. Toronto. Moscow. New York. Miami. Los Angeles. Istanbul. Singapore. Hong Kong.

Functional connectivity and language impairment in ...
Aug 3, 2010 - impairment, activation maps and functional connectivity networks were studied by fMRI of language. ... For the word-generation paradigm, patients with epilepsy had ...... Levels of evidence: taking Neurology® to the next level.

Stable communication through dynamic language - Semantic Scholar
texts in which particular words are used, or the way in which they are ... rules of grammar can only be successfully transmit- ted if the ... are much more likely to pass through the bottleneck into the ... ternal world is not sufficient to avoid the

Stable communication through dynamic language - Semantic Scholar
In E. Briscoe, editor, Linguistic. Evolution through Language Acquisition: Formal and Computational Models, pages 173–203. Cam- bridge University Press ...

COMMITTEE CONNECTIVITY IN THE UNITED ... - Semantic Scholar
that has jurisdiction over taxation, trade and several entitlement programs, and in- ... disproportionately assigned to certain committees, such as Small Business and ...... mittee connectivity after accounting for exclusive committee assignments.

Connectivity
Broadband service for 2 years, provided by Verizon Wireless. Also available are an unlimited day pass for just $9.99 and pay-as-you-go rates that are.

Constrained synaptic connectivity in functional ...
neuronal activity by forming artificial neuronal circuits using neuron to electronic ... us to obtain mature and durable neural networks of controlled architecture.

Constrained synaptic connectivity in functional ...
The use of ordered neuronal networks in vitro is a promising approach to study the development and the activity of small ... Hippocampal neurons in these networks are maintained healthy in serum free medium up ... It may be then feasible to monitor a

Effective connectivity: Influence, causality and ...
Available online xxxx. Keywords: .... whether some causal link is likely to be present (by comparing models with and ...... This simple model has proven to be a useful tool in many fields, including ...... Human Brain Mapping annual Meeting.

Consensus, cohesion and connectivity
Jun 23, 2017 - ity increases the predictive power of social influence theory, shown by re-using experimental data ... sciences—social cohesion (Section 4)—that was defined consider- ing a multiplicity of independent ..... but in actuality there a

Altered connectivity between prefrontal and ...
disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). .... The stimuli were presented using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral systems,. 153 ...... Cavada, C., Company, T., Tejedor, J., Cruz-Rizzolo, R. J., & Reinoso-Suarez, F. (2000). 494.

The Projection Dynamic and the Replicator Dynamic
Feb 1, 2008 - and the Replicator Dynamic. ∗. William H. Sandholm†, Emin Dokumacı‡, and Ratul Lahkar§ ...... f ◦H−1. Since X is a portion of a sphere centered at the origin, the tangent space of X at x is the subspace TX(x) = {z ∈ Rn : x