www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 36 (2007) 684 – 690

The relationship within and between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems indicated by resting state correlational patterns of sensory cortices Lixia Tian, a,b Tianzi Jiang, b,⁎ Yong Liu, b Chunshui Yu, c Kun Wang, b Yuan Zhou, b Ming Song, b and Kuncheng Li c a

Department of Biomedical Engineering, School of Computer and Information Technology, Beijing Jiaotong University, Beijing 100044, PR China National Laboratory of Pattern Recognition, Institute of Automation, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100080, PR China c Department of Radiology, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital University of Medical Sciences, Beijing 100053, PR China b

Received 31 October 2006; revised 16 February 2007; accepted 8 March 2007 Available online 1 April 2007 Much Research has been done on extrinsic and intrinsic systems, which consist of brain regions associated with the processing of externally and internally oriented stimuli, respectively. However, understanding of the underlying relationships within and between these two systems is relatively limited. To improve our understanding of these underlying relationships, we investigated the positive and negative correlations of three regions of interest (ROIs) located in the auditory, visual and somatosensory systems by using resting state functional MRI (fMRI) with a large sample size. We found that all three sensory systems exhibited significant negative correlation with the intrinsic system. In contrast, positive correlations between these sensory cortices and brain regions outside their respective system were limited. The present study extended former findings by indicating that multiple subsystems rather than a single subsystem of the extrinsic system are inherently negatively correlated with the intrinsic system. We suggest that these negative correlations between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems may explain the phenomenon that externally and internally oriented processes can always disturb or even interrupt each other. © 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Negative correlation; Default mode network; Functional MRI (fMRI)

Introduction Functional neuroimaging techniques, such as positron emission tomography (PET) and functional MRI (fMRI), have been widely used to investigate the mechanisms of human brain functions such as perception and cognition. Typically a task is presented and the brain regions involved in the task can be

⁎ Corresponding author. Fax: +86 10 6255 1993. E-mail address: [email protected] (T. Jiang). Available online on ScienceDirect (www.sciencedirect.com). 1053-8119/$ - see front matter © 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.044

obtained by evaluating the responses recorded by functional neuroimaging. An enormous amount of information has been accumulated about the systems specialized for externally oriented processes, such as perceptual processing (e.g., the auditory, visual and somatosensory systems) and exogenous cognitive processing (e.g., the “task-positive” network defined by Fox et al., 2005). In the present study, the brain regions associated with the processing of external inputs will be referred to as the “extrinsic system” to use the terminology of Golland et al. (2007). Accordingly, the auditory, visual and somatosensory systems as well as the “taskpositive” network could be seen as subsystems of the extrinsic system. Despite the fact that the majority of functional neuroimaging studies investigate externally oriented processes, several recent studies have begun to investigate internally oriented processes by using specially designed tasks (Kjaer et al., 2002; Lou et al., 2004; Decety and Sommerville, 2003). Interest in these internally oriented processes was sparked by the finding that a particular set of brain regions was deactivated during high-demanding cognitive task performances (Shulman et al., 1997; Gusnard and Raichle, 2001; Simpson et al., 2001; Mazoyer et al., 2001). This set of brain regions includes the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC)/precuneus, the medial prefrontal cortex (MedFC), the bilateral inferior parietal lobes (IPL), etc. These deactivations are believed to result from the interruption of the internally oriented processes by external stimuli (Shulman et al., 1997; for a review, see Gusnard and Raichle, 2001). Based on their PET study, Raichle et al. (2001) suggested that this set of brain regions may support a default mode of human brain function during the resting state, when the internally oriented processes are likely to occur. In the present study, those brain regions within the “default mode” network will frequently be referred to as the “intrinsic system” because of their association with internally oriented processing, in accordance with the terminology of Golland et al. (2007).

L. Tian et al. / NeuroImage 36 (2007) 684–690

A few recent resting state fMRI studies reported intrinsic correlations within and between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems. Specifically, positive correlations of slow spontaneous fluctuations in the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signals have been found within both the default mode network (Greicius et al., 2003; Fox et al., 2005; Fransson, 2005) and the subsystems of the extrinsic system, including the motor (Biswal et al., 1995; Lowe et al., 1998), visual (Cordes et al., 2000), auditory (Cordes et al., 2000), language (Hampson et al., 2002) and dorsal and ventral attention (Fox et al., 2006) systems. Greicius et al. (2003) first noticed the negative correlations of resting state spontaneous fluctuations. Fox et al. (2005) and Fransson (2005) expanded the findings by Greicius et al. (2003) by indicating that significant negative correlations of spontaneous fluctuations exist between the “task-positive” network, a subsystem of the extrinsic system, and the “task-negative” network, which was essentially the intrinsic system. These resting state functional network patterns have been found to be quite consistent across subjects (Beckmann et al., 2005; DeLuca et al., 2006; Damoiseaux et al., 2006; Fox et al., 2005) suggesting that the positive and negative correlations within and between systems may reflect the intrinsic human brain functional organization. Previous findings relevant to the positive and negative correlations associated with the sensory systems, especially those relevant to the negative correlations, seem to be quite inconsistent. Fox et al. (2005) found neither the “task-positive” network nor the “task-negative” network was correlated with the sensory cortices during the resting state. The resting state study by Cordes et al. (2000) did not report significant negative correlations associated with the sensory cortices while analyzing their correlational patterns. The study by Greicius and Menon (2004) provided some clues for the existence of negative correlations between the sensory systems and the default mode network: the strength and size of the subjects’ activation to auditory and visual stimuli was found to be closely related to the extent of the default mode network being suppressed. In the study by Tian et al. (2007), the negative correlations associated with the sensory cortices were found to be unstable. In addition to the differences in the processing methods and the states used (e.g., resting state Fox et al., 2005; Tian et al., in press; and task performance Greicius and Menon, 2004), we suggest that the relatively small sample size adopted in these studies may also be a factor that introduced these inconsistencies since individual variations are expected to have a significant influence on the statistical results when the sample size is relatively small. In the present study, we evaluated the positive and negative correlation of three representative sensory cortices, namely, the auditory, somatosensory and visual cortices using a large sample size to investigate whether the sensory systems are inherently correlated with other subsystems of the extrinsic system, and whether they are inherently negatively correlated with the intrinsic system. We chose to use a large sample size to diminish the influence of individual variations and thus to avoid any possible instabilities of the correlations of BOLD signals that could possibly occur when the sample size was comparatively small (Tian et al., in press). Indeed, the large sample size employed in the present study enabled us to gain new insights into the relationships within and between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems despite the resemblance in states (resting state) and methods (ROI-based correlation analyses) between Tian et al. (in press) and the present study.

685

Materials and methods Subjects and imaging methods Eighty-three normal subjects (37 females, 23.7 ± 3.8 years) participated in the present study after giving written informed consent in accordance with Xuanwu Hospital’s Review Board. All the subjects were recruited by advertisement. Due to excessive head motions, the functional images of 4 subjects were excluded from further analyses. All imaging was performed on a SIEMENS TRIO 3-T scanner at the Department of Radiology, Xuanwu Hospital. Each subject underwent a 9-min scan during a conscious resting state. Functional images were collected axially using an echo-planar imaging sequence sensitive to BOLD contrast. The acquisition parameters were 32 slices, 2000/30 ms (TR/TE), 3.0/1.0 mm (thickness/gap), 220 × 220 mm (FOV), 64 × 64 (resolution), 90° (flip angle). Other sessions not used in the present study will not be described here. During the resting state scanning, the subjects were instructed to keep still with their eyes closed, as motionless as possible and not to think about anything in particular. Data preprocessing The first 10 volumes were discarded to reduce the influence of the scanner instabilities and to allow for subjects’ adaptation to the environment. The remaining functional scans were first corrected for within-scan acquisition time differences between slices and then realigned to the first volume to correct for within-run head motions. This realigning step provided a record of head motions within each fMRI run. Subsequently, the functional scans were spatially normalized to a standard template (Montreal Neurological Institute) and resampled to 3 × 3 × 3 mm3. All these processes were conducted using SPM2 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Three possible sources of artifacts were then removed from the data through linear regression (Fox et al., 2005; Salvador et al., 2005; Golland et al., 2007). These were (1) 6 head motion parameters obtained in the realigning step, (2) the whole brain signal obtained by averaging the time series of all the voxels within the brain and (3) the linear trend. Finally, the waveform of each voxel was temporally band-pass filtered (0.01–0.08 Hz) using AFNI (http:// afni.nimh.nih.gov/) to reduce the influences of low-frequency drift and high-frequency noise. Since correlational analysis is sensitive to head motions, the data sets with the maximum displacement in any cardinal direction (x, y, z) greater than 1 mm were discarded. As mentioned above, images of only 79 subjects were available for later analyses. Correlational analysis In the present study, we examined the positive and negative correlations of the auditory, somatosensory and visual cortices using an ROI-based method. ROIs for the auditory, somatosensory and visual cortices were defined as the 6-mm-radius spheres centered in the left AI (Talairach coordinates [− 56, −17, 6]), left SI (Talairach coordinates [− 47, − 31, 55]) and left VII (Talairach coordinates [− 27, − 84, − 2]), respectively. The centres for the auditory and visual ROIs were decided according to the activation peaks in a previous study (Tian et al., in press), and the one for the somatosensory ROI was decided according to the peak identified in

686

L. Tian et al. / NeuroImage 36 (2007) 684–690

another study (Blankenburg et al., 2003). Illustrations of the location of the ROIs can be found in Fig. S1 in the supplementary materials on the NeuroImage Web site. Seed reference time courses were obtained by averaging the time series of all voxels in the ROIs. To obtain subject-specific correlation maps for each ROI, Pearson’s correlation analysis was performed between the specific seed reference time course and time series from the whole brain on a voxel-wise basis. This produced spatial maps in which the values of the voxels represented the strength of the correlation with the ROIs. Finally, the correlation coefficients were transformed into z-scores using Fisher’s transformation to improve normality (Press et al., 1992).

within the bilateral auditory cortices (60048 mm3) were 13.5 times larger than that in the SMA (4131 mm3). The left SI was also positively correlated with two frontal regions (Figs. 1c–d, Table 1), while the cluster size in the bilateral somatosensory cortices (60750 mm3) was 10.0 times that in the frontal lobe (6048 mm3). It should be noted that when the threshold was lowered, more positive correlations between these sensory cortices and brain regions outside their respective system would occur (please refer to Fig. S2 in the supplementary materials on the NeuroImage Web site to find the positive correlation maps thresholded at P < 0.05 (FWE corrected) and cluster size > 2700 mm3). Negative correlations

Statistical analysis A random-effect (Holmes and Friston, 1998) one-tailed onesample t-test was performed to obtain within-group positive and negative correlation maps. The random-effect analysis estimates the error variance across subjects, rather than across scans, and thus allows the present results to be generalized to normal populations. A large sample size will allow a stringent threshold for the following X¯  A two reasons: (1) It enhances the t-score (t ¼ pffiffiffi0 ; t will increase S= n

when the sample size n is increased). (2) It enhances the degrees of freedom and thus a lower p-value can be obtained for the same t-score. In the present study, the large sample size enabled us to choose a threshold of P < 0.00001 (FWE corrected) and cluster size > 2700 mm3 (100 resampled voxels). Statistical maps were displayed by superimposing the statistical results onto a glass brain as well as rendering these results onto a 3D brain reconstruction provided by SPM2 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Results Positive correlations Each ROI was strongly coupled with its contralateral counterpart (Table 1, Fig. 1d). In addition, positive correlations between these sensory cortices and brain regions outside their respective system were relatively limited in extent. Specifically, the left VII had no positive correlation with brain regions outside the visual system. The left AI was also positively correlated with the supplementary motor area (SMA, BA6) (Figs. 1a–b, Table 1), a component of the “task-positive” network, while the clusters Table 1 Significant positive correlations with the sensory cortices during resting state ROI Left AI

Region

BA Cluster size, mm3

L AI/AII/Insula 30,564 R AI/AII/Insula 29,484 B SMA 4131 Left SI L SI/SII/IPL 33,480 R SI/SII/IPL 27,270 L IFG 3132 L MFG 2916 Left VII B VI/VAII/VIII 122,580

41/22/42/13 41/22/42/13 6 1/2/3/40 1/2/3/40 44 46 17/18/19

Talairach (peak)

t-score (peak)

− 56 −17 6 62 − 8 6 − 3 − 3 50 − 50 −29 54 50 − 29 54 − 50 7 22 − 45 39 20 − 27 −87 2

29.80 14.40 11.29 29.56 15.90 10.61 9.61 29.51

Height threshold was P < 0.00001 (FEW corrected); extent threshold was cluster size >2700 mm3. Abbreviations: L—left, R—right, B—bilateral, BA—Brodmann's area, SMA—supplementary motor area, IPL—inferior parietal lobe, IFG—inferior frontal gyrus, MFG—middle frontal gyrus.

All three negative correlation maps shared some resemblance to the default mode network (Fig. 2, Table 2). Specifically, the left VII was negatively correlated with all three representative components of the default mode network, the (PCC)/precuneus, the MedFC and the bilateral IPL (Figs. 2e–f, Table 2). In addition to these four brain regions, the bilateral superior frontal gyrus in symmetric positions was also negatively correlated with the left VII (Figs. 2e–f). Besides the three representative components of the default mode network, the left SI was also negatively correlated with symmetric areas of the bilateral cerebellum (Figs. 2c–d, Table 2). The left AI showed significant negative correlation only with two of the three representative components of the default mode network, the PCC/ precuneus and the left IPL (Figs. 2a–b, Table 2). In addition, when the threshold was lowered, a more complete default mode network appeared in the negative correlation maps (please refer to Fig. S3 in the supplementary materials on the NeuroImage Web site to find the negative correlation maps thresholded at P < 0.05 (FWE corrected) and cluster size > 2700 mm3), and more obvious similarities between these negative correlation maps also occurred (please refer to Fig. S4 in the supplementary materials on the NeuroImage Web site to find the overlap maps of the three negative correlation maps). Discussion To investigate whether the sensory systems are inherently correlated with other subsystems of the extrinsic system, and whether they are inherently negatively correlated with the intrinsic system, we evaluated the positive and negative correlations of three sensory cortices with a large sample size using resting state fMRI. A large sample size enabled us to largely diminish the influences of individual variations that may have significant influence on statistical results when the sample size is relatively small (Tian et al., in press). Moreover, as has been mentioned, a large sample size can enhance the t-score as well as the degrees of freedom and ultimately lead to a stringent threshold. In the present study, the large sample size enabled us to employ a threshold of P < 0.00001 (FWE corrected) and cluster size >2700 mm3. Under this threshold, the clusters within the correlation maps were neither too widespread nor too limited. More importantly, the positive correlation maps obtained under this threshold were consistent not only with those obtained using ROI-based correlational analysis (Cordes et al., 2000; Tian et al., in press), but also with those obtained using probabilistic independent component analysis (Beckmann et al., 2005; Damoiseaux et al., 2006). A number of correlations between the subsystems of the extrinsic system could only survive a lowered threshold (Fig. S2).

L. Tian et al. / NeuroImage 36 (2007) 684–690

687

Fig. 1. Positive correlation maps of the auditory (a–b), somatosensory (c–d) and visual (e–f) cortices during conscious resting state. The statistical results were displayed within a glass brain (a, c, e) as well as rendered onto a 3D brain reconstruction (b, d, f) provided by SPM2. The height threshold was P < 0.00001 (FWE corrected) and the extent threshold was cluster size >2700 mm3.

In other words, compared to the correlations within subsystems as well as those between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems, those between the subsystems of the extrinsic system seem to be limited in extent, at least, when using the present stringent threshold. Specifically, the visual system was not positively correlated with any other subsystems of the extrinsic system (Figs. 1e–f, Table 1). In the auditory correlation map (Figs. 1a–b, Table 1), the size of clusters within the auditory cortices occupied a relatively large percent of the volume of the auditory system, while the cluster size of the SMA occupied a quite small percent of the overall volume of other subsystems of the extrinsic system. Similarly, in the somatosensory correlation map (Figs. 1c–d, Table 1), the size of clusters within the frontal regions also occupied a quite small percent of the overall volume of other subsystems of the extrinsic system. Considering the enormous functional interactions within subsystems as well as those between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems, as reflected by their extensive correlations, we suggest that the interactions between the subsystems of the extrinsic system seem to be relatively limited.

All three sensory systems were negatively correlated with the default mode network (Fig. 2, Fig. S3, Fig. S4, Table 2). Among these three systems, the bilateral SII were the only brain regions that have been reported to be intrinsically anti-correlated with the default mode network (Fransson, 2005). However, the study by Greicius and Menon (2004), although carried out during task performance, indirectly provided evidence of the intrinsic anti-correlations between the default mode network and the auditory and visual systems. In their study, the extent of the default mode activity being suppressed was found to be positively correlated with the height and extent of the activation of the auditory and visual cortices (Greicius and Menon, 2004). As has been mentioned, Fox et al. (2005) and Fransson (2005) found only significant negative correlations between the “taskpositive” network and the default mode network. The present study extended the previous findings (Greicius et al., 2003; Fox et al., 2005; Fransson, 2005) by indicating that multiple subsystems, rather than a single subsystem, of the extrinsic system are negatively correlated with the intrinsic system. In accordance with

688

L. Tian et al. / NeuroImage 36 (2007) 684–690

Fig. 2. Negative correlation maps of the auditory (a–b), somatosensory (c–d) and visual (e–f) cortices during conscious resting state. The statistical results were displayed within a glass brain (a, c, e) as well as rendered onto a 3D brain reconstruction (b, d, f) provided by SPM2. The height threshold was P < 0.00001 (FWE corrected) and the extent threshold was cluster size >2700 mm3.

the suggestion that negative correlations may play a differentiating or competing role (Fox et al., 2005), we suggest that these widespread negative correlations between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems may facilitate the functional competition, or at least differentiation, between these two systems. This suggestion is consistent with our daily experience in which externally and internally oriented processes can always disturb or even interrupt each other. Specifically, external stimuli can often diminish internally oriented processes that have been suggested to be the role of the default mode network, such as general information gathering and evaluation (Raichle et al., 2001; Gusnard and Raichle, 2001), task-independent thought (Binder et al., 1999; McKiernan et al., 2006), episodic memory (Greicius et al., 2003) and self-referential processing (Northoff and Bermpohl, 2004). Similarly, internally oriented processes can also disturb externally oriented processes (Teasdale et al., 1995; Giambra, 1995; McKiernan et al., 2003). Considering the intrinsic negative correlations between the sensory systems and the default mode network, one might expect

that sensory task performance would lead to the default mode network deactivation. However, to our knowledge, no previous study reported default mode deactivations during sensory task performance. This inconsistency can be explained by the study by Greicius and Menon (2004). In their study, both the strength and size of the activations in the auditory and visual cortices were found to increase with the suppression of the default mode activity, while little to no deactivation was found in the default mode network. Based on their findings, Greicius and Menon (2004) concluded that “not all tasks are sufficiently engaging to disrupt the default mode network”, that is, such factors as task difficulty and task novelty can also influence default mode network deactivation. Two important issues with respect to the present finding of significant negative correlations between the sensory systems and the default mode network still need to be addressed. One issue is that the present finding of significant negative correlations between the sensory systems and the default mode network seems to be inconsistent with a few previous resting state fMRI studies. First,

L. Tian et al. / NeuroImage 36 (2007) 684–690 Table 2 Significant negative correlations with the sensory cortices during resting state ROI

Region

Left AI

B PCC/precuneus 12,852 L IPL 3807 B PCC/precuneus 22,950 B MedFC/ACC 26,703

Left SI

L IPL R IPL R Cerebellum L Cerebellum Left VII L IPL R IPL L SFG B MedFC R SFG B PCC

Cluster BA size, mm3

6372 3969 5346 5346 13,014 13,068 4293 5778 5535 3051

23/7 39 23/31/7 8/10/ 11/32 39/40 39/40

39/40 39/40 8 8/9 8 23

Talairach (peak)

t-score (peak)

0 −56 39 − 45 −68 39 − 9 − 48 33 0 56 19

11.93 9.45 13.89 13.35

− 45 −57 33 56 − 60 33 30 − 86 − 23 − 24 −86 −23 − 59 −51 36 45 − 68 45 − 24 23 51 − 3 45 31 36 17 52 0 −39 41

11.31 10.27 15.76 12.98 14.38 13.90 10.77 10.42 10.42 9.32

Height threshold was P < 0.00001 (FEW corrected); extent threshold was cluster size >2700 mm3. Abbreviations: L—left, R—right, B—bilateral, BA—Brodmann's area, PCC—posterior cingulate cortex, IPL—inferior parietal lobe, ACC—anterior cingulate cortex, MedFC—medial frontal cortex, SFG—superior frontal gyrus.

former resting state fMRI studies using the same sensory cortices as ROIs as we used did not report significant negative correlations associated with them (Cordes et al., 2000, 2001). We suggest that this may have primarily been caused by the fact that whole brain signals, which were not removed in the studies by Cordes et al. (2000, 2001), can enhance the correlations throughout the entire brain (Laurienti, 2004). Secondly, in our former study (Tian et al., in press), no consistent negative correlations were observed between auditory, visual systems and the default mode network. We suggest that this could have resulted from individual variations within these negative correlations. In the present study we used a large sample size to decrease the influence of individual variations and thus obtained significant negative correlations. Thirdly, in the study by Fox et al. (2005), the default mode network was found to have no significant correlation with any sensory system. We suggest that this may have been caused by individual variations in these negative correlations, since the fixed-effect analysis method used in the study would be sensitive to individual variations (Holmes and Friston, 1998). Moreover, the specific choice of ROIcentres may also influence the ultimate negative correlational patterns. The second issue that needs to be addressed is why the subsystems of the extrinsic system exhibited limited positive correlation with each other, considering our finding that they each negatively correlated with the default mode network. Numerous functional connectivity analyses have demonstrated that the correlational relationship was not a transferable one (e.g., Greicius et al., 2003). This means that significant correlation between brain regions “A” and “B”, as well as between “B” and “C” does not necessarily lead to significant correlation between brain regions “A” and “C”. It is certainly possible that correlation between brain regions “A” and “C” might survive a lowered threshold considering that they both significantly correlated with brain region “B”. Applying the above rule, that they are not necessarily correlated, it would be reasonable to expect that the subsystems of

689

the extrinsic system were not found to be correlated with each other at the current stringent threshold, though they each exhibited significant negative correlation with the default mode network. Overall, by evaluating the correlational patterns of three sensory ROIs based on a large sample size, we found that the positive correlations associated with each sensory system were relatively limited in extent, while each sensory system exhibited significant negative correlations with the default mode network. The present study extended former findings (Greicius et al., 2003; Fox et al., 2005; Fransson, 2005) by indicating that multiple subsystems, rather than a single subsystem, of the extrinsic system are inherently anticorrelated with the intrinsic system. We suggest that these negative correlations between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems may reflect the inherent functional competition or differentiation between them. In addition, we suggest that the functional interactions between the subsystems of the extrinsic system are relatively limited compared to those within subsystems as well as those between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems. Acknowledgments The authors are grateful to the anonymous referees for their significant and constructive comments and suggestions, which greatly improved the paper. The authors also thank Drs. Rhoda E. and Edmund F. Perozzi for editing and English language assistance. This work was partially supported by the Natural Science Foundation of China, Grant Nos. 30425004, 30570509 and 60121302, the National Key Basic Research and Development Program (973) Grant No. 2003CB716100 and the Beijing Jiaotong University Rencai Funding, Grant Nos. 2006RC043. Appendix A. Supplementary data Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.044. References Beckmann, C.F., DeLuca, M., Devlin, J.T., Smith, S.M., 2005. Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B Biol. Sci. 360, 1001–1013. Binder, J.R., Frost, J.A., Hammeke, T.A., Bellgowan, P.S.F., Rao, S.M., Cox, R., 1999. Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: a functional MRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 80–93. Biswal, B., Yetkin, F.Z., Haughton, V.M., Hyde, J.S., 1995. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echoplanar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. Blankenburg, F., Ruben, J., Meyer, R., Schwiemann, J., Villringer, A., 2003. Evidence for a rostral-to-caudal somatotopic organization in human primary somatosensory cortex with mirror-reversal in areas 3b and 1. Cereb. Cortex 13, 987–993. Cordes, D., Haughton, V.M., Arfanakis, K., Wendt, G.J., Turski, P.A., Moritz, C.H., Quigley, M.A., Meyerand, M.E., 2000. Mapping functionally related regions of brain with functional connectivity MR imaging. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 21, 1636–1644. Cordes, D., Haughton, V.M., Arfanakis, K., Carew, J.D., Turski, P.A., Moritz, C.H., Quigley, M.A., Meyerand, M.E., 2001. Frequencies contributing to functional connectivity in the cerebral cortex in “restingstate” data. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 22, 1326–1333. Damoiseaux, J.S., Rombouts, S.A.R.B., Barkhof, F., Scheltens, P., Stam, C.J., Smith, S.M., Beckmann, C.F., 2006. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 103, 13848–13853.

690

L. Tian et al. / NeuroImage 36 (2007) 684–690

Decety, J., Sommerville, J.A., 2003. Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 527–533. DeLuca, M., Beckmann, C.F., De Stefano, N., Matthews, P.M., Smith, S.M., 2006. FMRI resting state networks define distinct modes of longdistance interactions in the human brain. NeuroImage 29, 1359–1367. Fox, M.D., Snyder, A.Z., Vincent, J.L., Corbetta, M., Essen, D.C.V., Raichle, M.E., 2005. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 102, 9673–9678. Fox, M.D., Snyder, A.Z., Zacks, J.M., Raichle, M.E., 2006. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 103, 10046–10051. Fransson, P., 2005. Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations —an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26, 15–29. Giambra, L.M., 1995. A laboratory method for investigating influences on switching attention to task-unrelated imagery and thought. Conscious. Cogn. 4, 1–21. Golland, Y., Bentin, S., Gelbard, H., Benjamini, Y., Heller, R., Nir, Y., Hansson, U., Malach, R., 2007. Extrinsic and intrinsic systems in the posterior cortex of the human brain revealed during natural sensory stimulation. Cereb. Cortex 17, 766–777. Greicius, M.D., Menon, V., 2004. Default-mode activity during a passive sensory task: uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1484–1492. Greicius, M.D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A.L., Menon, V., 2003. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 100, 253–258. Gusnard, D.A., Raichle, M.E., 2001. Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat. Rev., Neurosci. 2, 685–694. Hampson, M., Peterson, B.S., Skudlarski, P., Gatenby, J.C., Gore, J.C., 2002. Detection of functional connectivity using temporal correlations in MR images. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 247–262. Holmes, A.P., Friston, K.J., 1998. Generalisability, random effects and population inference. NeuroImage 7, S754. Kjaer, T.W., Nowak, M., Lou, H.C., 2002. Reflective self-awareness and conscious states: PET evidence for a common midline parietofrontal core. NeuroImage 17, 1080–1086. Laurienti, P.J., 2004. Deactivations, global signal, and the default mode of brain function. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1481–1483. Lou, H.C., Luber, B., Crupain, M., Keenan, J.P., Nowak, M., Kjaer, T.W.,

Sackeim, H.A., Lisanby, S.H., 2004. Parietal cortex and representation of the mental self. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 6827–6832. Lowe, M.J., Mock, B.J., Sorenson, J.A., 1998. Functional connectivity in single and multislice echoplanar imaging using resting-state fluctuations. NeuroImage 7, 119–132. Mazoyer, B., Zago, L., Mellet, E., Bricogne, S., Etard, O., Houde, O., Crivello, F., Joliot, M., Petit, L., Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., 2001. Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Res. Bull. 54, 287–298. McKiernan, K.A., Kaufman, J.N., Kucera-Thompson, J., Binder, J.R., 2003. A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 394–408. McKiernan, K.A., D’Angelo, B.R., Kaufman, J.N., Binder, J.R., 2006. Interrupting the “stream of consciousness”: an fMRI investigation. NeuroImage 29, 1185–1191. Northoff, G., Bermpohl, F., 2004. Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 102–107. Press, W.H., Teukolsky, S.A., Vetterling, W.T., Flannery, B.P., 1992. Numerical Recipes in C, 2 ed. Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, p. 637. Raichle, M.E., MacLeod, A.M., Snyder, A.Z., Powers, W.J., Gusnard, D.A., Shulman, G.L., 2001. A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 98, 676–682. Salvador, R., Suckling, J., Schwarzbauer, C., Bullmore, E., 2005. Undirected graphs of frequency-dependent functional connectivity in whole brain networks. Philos. T. Roy. Soc. B 360, 937–946. Shulman, G.L., Fiez, J.A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R.L., Miezin, F.M., Raichle, M.E., Petersen, S.E., 1997. Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 648–663. Simpson, J.R.J., Snyder, A.Z., Gusnard, D.A., Raichle, M.E., 2001. Emotion-induced changes in human medial prefrontal cortex: I. During cognitive task performance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 98, 683–687. Teasdale, J.D., Dritschel, B.H., Taylor, M.J., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C.A., Nimmo-Smith, I., Baddeley, A.D., 1995. Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources. Mem. Cogn. 23, 551–559. Tian, L., Jiang, T., Liang, M., Li, X., He, Y., Wang, K., Cao, B., Jiang, T., in press. Stabilities of negative correlations between blood oxygen leveldependent signals associated with sensory and motor cortices. Hum Brain Mapp.

The relationship within and between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems ...

Apr 1, 2007 - aDepartment of Biomedical Engineering, School of Computer and Information .... system. These resting state functional network patterns have been .... peaks in a previous study (Tian et al., in press), and the one for the.

821KB Sizes 2 Downloads 413 Views

Recommend Documents

The relationship within and between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems ...
Apr 1, 2007 - 360, 1001–1013. Binder, J.R., Frost, J.A., Hammeke, T.A., ... other: a social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7,. 527–533.

Unravelling extrinsic and intrinsic factors of the early ...
comprehensive database including all records of blastozoans was built to provide quantitative analyses of .... regional databases was tested by the comparison of their rarefied .... test (bottom triangular matrix) and Kendall correlation (top triangu

Prosody and literacy: the relationship between children's ...
Prosody and literacy: the relationship between children's suprasegmental representations and reading skills. Catherine Dickie. University of Edinburgh. Abstract. One major theory of developmental dyslexia argues that the literacy difficulties seen in

Thermal characterization of intrinsic and extrinsic InP ...
Apr 2, 2003 - attention in the context of fabrication of electronic and optoelectronic devices .... Introduction of a foreign atom into the host lattice creates more ...

intrinsic and extrinsic motivators to study industrial ...
proper focus group and make it comfortable for their students to express their feelings without .... I wanted to be a teacher but teachers do not earn money. V.

The Relationship Between the UNIVAC Computer and ... - IJEECS
Similarly, we show the diagram used by our heuristic in Figure 1. While futurists rarely assume the exact opposite, ... Intel 386s from the NSA's Internet-2 cluster to better understand our mobile telephones. We only characterized ... space of our se

The Relationship Between the UNIVAC Computer and ... - GitHub
Feb 20, 2014 - Abstract. Many electrical engineers would agree that, had it not been for online algorithms, the evaluation of red-black trees might never have ...

The Relationship Between the UNIVAC Computer and ... - GitHub
Apr 28, 2015 - Computer and Evolutionary Programming. Bob, Carol and Alice ... tiplayer online role-playing games and the location-identity split. We con-.

The Relationship Between the UNIVAC Computer and ... - IJEECS
X. JVM. Trap. Figure 1: An algorithm for atomic methodolo- gies. hurt. This may or may not actually hold in reality. See our prior technical report [19] for details. Similarly, we show the diagram used by our heuristic in Figure ... ware; and finally

The Relationship Between Degree of Bilingualism and ...
ous findings in that they suggest that bilingualism promotes an analytic ... to approach the cognitive tasks in a truly analytic way. .... One partial solution to both of ...

The Relationship Between Child Anthropometry and ...
would be missed by policies and programs focusing primarily or ... high mortality levels and that morbidity has its biggest impacts in ... collect and/or use ancillary data in the analysis. How ...... mit a test of their hypothesis that the malnutrit

On the Relationship Between Quality and Productivity - Personal ...
Aug 5, 2016 - Firms from developing countries historically have failed to break into developed country ...... “The Surprisingly Swift Decline of U.S. Manufac-.

On the relationship between Spearman's rho and Kendall's tau for ...
different aspects of the dependence structure. For example, if X and Y are random variables with marginal distribution functions F and G, respectively, then Spearman's is the ordinary (Pearson) correlation coefficient of the transformed random variab

The relationship between dewlap size and performance changes with ...
Received: 11 October 2004 / Accepted: 30 May 2005 / Published online: 23 August 2005 ... by multiple traits. (e.g. number of copulations, ability to acquire nest sites or to .... line represents the bimodal distribution based on the dip test (see tex

the relationship between institutional mission and ...
characteristics, no meaningful differences were found in students' ... and governing boards use performance-indicator systems that are based, in part,.

The relationship between foot arch measurements and ...
Jan 20, 2016 - With the GAITRite software, spatial and temporal parameters were calculated .... Separate multiple regression ana- lyses were conducted using ...

On the Relationship Between Quality and Productivity ...
We develop a simple model of output and input quality choice to flesh out the .... Finally, the Chinese import tariff data are obtained from the WTO website, ...

The Relationship Between Cholesterol and Survival in ...
teristic curves demonstrated a best cut-off value of 5.2 mmol/l (200.8 mg/dl) as being the best predictor of ... 11, 2003. © 2003 by the American College of Cardiology Foundation ... physician, or through the Hospital Information System in.

Nonlinearities in the relationship between debt and ...
Jan 5, 2017 - tionship between public debt and growth employing long time series data from up to. 27 countries. My empirical approach recognises that ...

The relationship between product and international ...
Nov 10, 2008 - Lally School of Management and Technology, Rensselaer Polytechnic ..... database. ... ally in Compustat's business segment file, product.

The Relationship between Personality Profile and ...
This article explores the relationship between Indira Gandhi's personality profile in the .... which her personality profile was predictive of her leadership style. ... of standard personality instruments and techniques to the analysis of biographi-

The relationship between growth and profit: evidence ...
Available online xxx. JEL classification: .... Furthermore, high growth may cause problems to young firms. ... 583 European banks to show that current profit is a pre- requisite for ... Of particular interest is a series of empirical studies conducte

Is the relationship between aid and economic growth ...
Apr 24, 2007 - H = (h1,h2,...,hb) where b is the number of regimes in T, a single tree model may be de- noted by the pair (T,H). Let ^fрwi ..... Growth database. Sachs and Warner, (1995). Openness. Average openness measure proposed by. Sachs and War

The relationship between temporal and spectral ...
The recruitment of participants was made via leaflets distributed to patients attending audiological services in Nottingham for a hearing assessment. Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the Derbyshire Research Ethics. Committee. All par