ARTICLE IN PRESS

G Model NSY 3597 1–7

Neuropsychologia xxx (2010) xxx–xxx 1

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuropsychologia

F

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia

3

Floris P. de Lange a,∗ , Ivan Toni a,c , Karin Roelofs b

4

a

5

b

6

c

a r t i c l e

i n f o

a b s t r a c t

9 11 12 13

Article history: Received 20 October 2009 Received in revised form 17 February 2010 Accepted 25 February 2010 Available online xxx

1. Introduction

19 20

23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39

Conversion paralysis (CP) is a psychiatric disorder that is characterized by a loss of voluntary motor functioning. Although the symptoms may suggest a neuropathological condition, they cannot be adequately explained by known neurological or other organic Q1 disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Moreover, the onset or exacerbation of symptoms is related to psychological stress, suggesting that psychological mechanisms play an important role (Roelofs & Spinhoven, 2007). Despite the high prevalence of CP and the long history of speculations about its cause (Halligan, Bass, & Marshall, 2001; Vuilleumier, 2005), the exact nature of CP remains poorly understood. Several neuroimaging studies have tried to explore the objective neural correlates of functional mechanisms that, in the absence of a structural brain lesion, may be involved in CP. These studies have generally implicated ventromedial (Halligan, Athwal, Oakley, & Frackowiak, 2000; Marshall, Halligan, Fink, Wade, & Frackowiak, 1997) and dorsolateral (Spence, Crimlisk, Cope, Ron, & Grasby,

CO

18

UN

17

EC

22

16

RR

21

Keywords: fMRI Mental rotation Motor imagery Conversion disorder Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex Connectivity

15

TE

14

Conversion paralysis (CP) is a frequent and impairing psychiatric disorder, affecting voluntary motor function. Yet, we have previously shown that the motor system of CP patients with a unilateral conversion paresis is recruited to a similar degree during imagined movements of the affected and unaffected limb. In contrast, imagery of movements with the affected limb results in larger prefrontal activation. It remains unclear how this hand-specific increased prefrontal activity relates to the reduced responsiveness of motor and somatosensory areas, a consistent and important feature of CP patients. In the current study, we investigated changes in the inter-regional coupling between prefrontal cortex (PFC) and sensorimotor regions when CP patients imagined movements involving either the affected or the unaffected hand. We found that there were distinct connectivity patterns for different parts of the PFC. While ventromedial PFC was not functionally connected to the motor system, we observed strong functional coupling between the dorsolateral PFC and various sensorimotor areas. Furthermore, this coupling was modulated by whether patients imagined movements of their affected or unaffected hand. Together, these results suggest that the reduced motor responsitivity observed in CP may be linked to altered dorsolateral prefrontal-motor connectivity. © 2010 Published by Elsevier Ltd.

D

10

PR

Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University Nijmegen, Kapittelweg 29, 6500 HB Nijmegen, Netherlands Leiden University-Institute of Psychology, Clinical, Health and Neuropsychology Unit, Netherlands Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition (LIBC), Netherlands

7

8

OO

2

Altered connectivity between prefrontal and sensorimotor cortex in conversion paralysis

1

∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +31 24 36 10658; fax: +31 24 36 10652. E-mail address: fl[email protected] (F.P. de Lange).

2000) regions within the prefrontal cortex during (failed) attempts of movements. Given the role of these regions in the internal generation of actions (Fuster, 2000; Passingham, 1993), these findings were interpreted as reflecting active motor inhibition. In most of these studies however, patients were explicitly asked to attempt to move their paralyzed limb, which may also trigger emotional and motivational responses, thus complicating the interpretation of the results (Price & Friston, 2002). To overcome the interpretational limitations imposed by overt motor behavior, some studies have studied cerebral responses evoked by passive sensory stimulation (Vuilleumier et al., 2001) or action observation (Burgmer et al., 2006). Vuilleumier et al. assessed brain responsiveness to passive sensory stimulation in CP patients suffering from unilateral sensorimotor loss in a single photon emission computerized tomography (SPECT) study. The results showed decreased activity in the basal ganglia and thalamus contralateral to the affected limb during stimulation of the affected limb compared to the unaffected limb. This decrease resolved after recovery of the conversion symptoms, suggesting that CP is associated with altered somatosensory processing. Burgmer et al. (2006) explored whether CP is associated with abnormal brain activity during observation of hand movements. The authors showed that compared to healthy controls, CP patients had reduced primary motor cortical

0028-3932/$ – see front matter © 2010 Published by Elsevier Ltd. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.029

Please cite this article in press as: de Lange, F. P., et al. Altered connectivity between prefrontal and sensorimotor cortex in conversion paralysis. Neuropsychologia (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.029

40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62

G Model NSY 3597 1–7

69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127

2.1. Participants

129

We studied eight patients (mean age of 34.6 years, range 18–56, SD = 13.2) diagnosed with conversion disorder according to the DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and showing a full or partial paralysis lateralized to one arm as a major symptom. For a full description of inclusion criteria and diagnosis procedure, see de Lange et al. (2007). The study was approved by the local medical ethical committee and all patients gave their informed consent before participation.

130

2.2. Task

136

We used a well-known motor imagery task, in which the participants have to judge the laterality of the visually presented rotated hand stimulus (Parsons, 1987). We used line drawings of left and right hands, in different orientations varying from 0◦ to 180◦ in 45◦ steps. The hand could be shown in either palmar or dorsal orientation. The stimuli were serially presented to the patients in a random order. For each trial, the hand stimulus was presented centrally on the screen, and patients were instructed to judge as fast and as accurately as possible whether the stimulus constituted a left or a right hand. When the patient provided his/her response, the stimulus was replaced with a fixation cross, which stayed on until the start of the next trial (inter-trial interval: 1.5–2.5 s). The experiment consisted of 160 trials of motor imagery. After a series of 10 motor imagery trials, a rest period of 10 s was introduced to sample baseline activity. During this rest period, patients were instructed to look at the fixation cross. Patients responded by pressing one of two buttons attached to their left and right big toe. The patients’ left and right feet were firmly attached to a button box, and reaction times and error rates were measured for subsequent behavioral analysis. The stimuli were presented using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral systems, Albany, USA), and they were projected onto a screen at the back of the scanner and seen through a mirror above the patients’ heads. The main effects of hand (affected vs. unaffected) and stimulus rotation on reaction times and cerebral activity have been described in a previous study on the same data set (de Lange et al., 2007). Here, we focus on functional and effective connectivity as a function of motor imagery of the affected vs. the unaffected hand, using the previously described prefrontal clusters as target seed regions.

137

2.3. MRI acquisition and preprocessing

161

Functional images were acquired on a Siemens (Erlangen, Germany) 1.5 T MRI system equipped with echo planar imaging (EPI) capabilities using the standard head coil for radio frequency transmission and signal reception. Functional images were acquired using a gradient EPI-sequence (TE/TR = 40/2540 ms; 32 axial slices, voxel size = 3.5 mm; FOV = 224 mm). On average, the duration of the experiment was 23 min in which 547 scans were acquired. High-resolution anatomical images were acquired using a MP-RAGE sequence (TE/TR = 3.93/2250 ms; voxel size = 1.0 mm, 176 sagittal slices; FOV = 256 mm). Preprocessing of the functional data and calculation of the contrast images for statistical analysis was done with SPM5 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). First, functional images were realigned, slice-time corrected, normalized to a common stereotactic space (MNI: Montreal Neurological Institute, Canada) and smoothed with a 10 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. Our spatial filter size was motivated by both the sizable inter-individual differences in prefrontal anatomy (Rajkowska & Goldman-Rakic, 1995) and in order to optimize sensitivity in the small sample of patients used (Petersson, Nichols, Poline, & Holmes, 1999). By jittering trial onsets with respect to image acquisition and randomizing stimulus rotations, our experimental design allowed for an event-related analysis of the fMRI time series.

162

2.4. Functional and effective connectivity analysis

180

We carried out an analysis of functional and effective connectivity, using the psychophysiological interaction (PPI) method to test for (changes in) connectivity between our seed regions and other brain areas (Friston et al., 1997). Functional connectivity ensues from significantly correlated activity patterns over time between separate regions. Effective connectivity, on the other hand, ensues from significant changes in correlation strength between separate regions as a function of task context. We placed seed regions in previously described clusters in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), namely the ventromedial PFC (vmPFC: [8,44,−24]; putative BA 11), dorsomedial PFC (dmPFC: [−12,62,32]; putative BA 10) and dorsolateral PFC (dlPFC: [−36,48,34]; putative BA 46/9). Each of these regions was defined by the first eigenvariate of the time series of all voxels within a 6 mm sphere surrounding the peak activations. These regions were chosen as they constituted the local maxima of differential activation between motor imagery of the affected vs. the unaffected hand (see de Lange et al., 2007). The PPI method makes inferences about regionally specific responses caused by the interaction between an experimentally manipulated psychological factor and the physiological activity measured in a given index area. The analysis was constructed to test for differences in the regression slope of PFC activity on other brain areas, depending on whether subjects performed motor imagery of the affected vs. the unaffected hand. To construct our PPI regressor we used a hemo-

181

F

68

128

OO

67

2. Materials and methods

ED

66

CT

65

activity during observation of hand movements, specifically for the affected hand. Although these findings do not suffer from the interpretational problems related to overt motor execution, it remains to be determined how these sensory and action observation deficits relate to the main feature of CP, namely the disturbance of volitional motor processes. A well-established approach to directly probe the motor system in absence of overt motor behavior makes use of motor imagery. This approach controls for neural processes associated with actual motor execution like altered sensory feedback or enhanced monitoring of failed movements. This approach to study the generation and preparation of actions is supported by a wealth of evidence showing that imagined and executed movements overlap in terms of time course (Parsons, 1987; Sekiyama, 1982), autonomic responses (Decety, Jeannerod, Durozard, & Baverel, 1993), and neural architecture (de Lange, Hagoort, & Toni, 2005; Jeannerod, 1994; Parsons, Gabrieli, Phelps, & Gazzaniga, 1998). Indeed, previous behavioral studies have succesfully used motor imagery tasks to reveal impairments in motoric simulations of the affected limb in patients with CP (Maruff & Velakoulis, 2000; Roelofs et al., 2001). Two imaging experiments using motor imagery in CP patients with a lateralized paresis extended these behavioral findings. The first study showed that while preparatory motor structures were activated similarly during motor imagery for the affected and the unaffected limb, there was larger activation in several prefrontal clusters during motor imagery of the affected limb (de Lange, Roelofs, & Toni, 2007). Given the role of these regions in selfreflexive processing (Goldberg, Harel, & Malach, 2006) as well as observation and awareness of actions (Castelli, Happe, Frith, & Frith, 2000; Frith, Blakemore, & Wolpert, 2000a), we speculated that these activation differences were due to heightened selfmonitoring of (imagined) actions with the affected arm. In a second study, we found that these activation differences disappeared when subjects were explicitly instructed to focus on the “to be imagined” movements (de Lange, Roelofs, & Toni, 2008), lending further support to the idea that increased self-monitoring of actions underlies the increased activation for imagery of actions with the affected arm. However, it remains unclear how the increased prefrontal responses relate to another consistent feature of CP patients, namely reduced responsiveness of motor and somatosensory areas. For instance, a recent study that directly probed the excitability of the motor cortex using transcranial magnetic stimulation observed striking excitability changes in the motor system during motor imagery of the affected hand in CP (Liepert, Hassa, Tüscher, & Schmidt, 2009). While motor imagery of the unaffected hand resulted in higher motor excitability, as it is normally observed in the healthy population (Abbruzzese, Trompetto, & Schieppati, 1996), motor imagery of the affected hand led to a paradoxical decrease of motor excitability. These results are in accord with other studies showing reduced responses in motor (Burgmer et al., 2006; Kanaan, Craig, Wessely, & David, 2007; Stone et al., 2007) and somatosensory areas (Ghaffar, Staines, & Feinstein, 2006; MailisGagnon et al., 2003) in CP. In this study, we used connectivity analyses to assess the relationship between local responses evoked in prefrontal and sensorimotor cortex during a motor imagery task involving either the affected or non-affected hand in CP (de Lange et al., 2007). We observed strong functional coupling between the dorsolateral PFC and various sensory and motor areas. Crucially, the strength of this coupling was modulated by whether patients imagined movements of their affected or unaffected hand. These results suggest that the reduced responsiveness of sensorimotor responses, previously observed in CP patients, could arise from altered connectivity between dorsolateral prefrontal and sensorimotor regions.

RE

64

F.P. de Lange et al. / Neuropsychologia xxx (2010) xxx–xxx

UN CO R

63

ARTICLE IN PRESS

PR

2

Please cite this article in press as: de Lange, F. P., et al. Altered connectivity between prefrontal and sensorimotor cortex in conversion paralysis. Neuropsychologia (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.029

131 132 133 134 135

138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160

163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179

182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199

G Model NSY 3597 1–7

ARTICLE IN PRESS F.P. de Lange et al. / Neuropsychologia xxx (2010) xxx–xxx

232

3. Results

233

We carried out analyses of functional and effective connectivity in three regions within the prefrontal cortex that were more active during motor imagery of affected than unaffected hands: ventromedial (vmPFC), dorsomedial (dmPFC) and dorsolateral (dlPFC) prefrontal cortex (Amodio & Frith, 2006). vmPFC was strongly functionally connected to the left ([−60,−10,−32]; T = 23.1, pFWE = 0.007) and right ([58,−14,−30]; T = 19.9, pFWE = 0.021) inferior temporal cortex. dmPFC showed significant functional connectivity with its contralateral counterpart ([18,60,34]; T = 38.2, pFWE < 0.001). These vmPFC and dmPFC clusters did not show significant coupling with any of the nodes of the sensorimotor network. Importantly, these clusters also did not show any significant changes in effective connectivity as a function of hand laterality.

210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224

228 229 230

234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245

EC

209

RR

208

3.1. Increased positive coupling between dlPFC and dorsal premotor cortex

263

We observed larger positive coupling for motor imagery of the affected hand compared to the unaffected hand between the dlPFC and the right dorsal premotor cortex (PMd). Although larger coupling could also be seen between dlPFC and the left PMd ([−18,8,64]; T = 4.33, puncorrected = 0.002), this result failed to reach significance when correcting for multiple comparisons within the predefined search space. Both left and right PMd showed overall significant increases in cerebral activity with increasing biomechanical complexity (left PMd: T = 8.94, pFWE = 0.0078; right PMd: T = 7.07, pFWE = 0.0034). There were no other brain regions that showed significantly larger positive coupling for motor imagery of the affected hand compared to the unaffected hand.

265

3.2. Increased negative coupling between dlPFC and primary somatosensory cortex and hippocampus

278

There were several regions that showed a significantly more negative regression slope as a function of dlPFC activity during motor imagery of the affected hand, compared to the unaffected hand. There was increased negative coupling in the left and right postcentral gyrus [spanning both BA 2 with 40% probability and BA 3a with 60% probability (Grefkes, Geyer, Schormann, Roland, & Zilles, 2001), and the anterior part of the superior parietal lobule].

280

CO

207

UN

206

F

231

Anatomical details of significant signal changes were obtained by superimposing the relevant SPMs on the structural images of the subjects. The atlas of Duvernoy, Cabanis, and Vannson (1991) was used to identify relevant anatomical landmarks. When applicable, Brodmann areas (BAs) were assigned on the basis of the SPM Anatomy Toolbox (Eickhoff et al., 2005).

205

OO

227

204

PR

2.5. Anatomical inference

203

246

D

226

202

In contrast, dlPFC showed strong functional coupling with several nodes of the sensorimotor system (Fig. 1). This analysis identified a network of regions that comprised the supramarginal gyrus [assigned to parietal area PF with 70% probability (Caspers et al., 2006; Eickhoff et al., 2005)], supplementary motor area and the central sulcus, encompassing both the precentral gyrus (assigned to area 3b with 60% probability) and postcentral gyrus [assigned to area 1 with 70% probability (Geyer, Schleicher, & Zilles, 1999)]. The precise anatomical localization of these clusters is provided in Table 1. The dense functional coupling between the dlPFC region and these sensorimotor regions, which are all involved in aspects of generation and planning of motor commands, is in good correspondence with the hypothesized role of the dlPFC seed region in the selection of action plans (Frith, Moody, & Driver, 2000b). We next investigated which regions showed stronger positive or negative coupling with the dlPFC as a function of whether subjects imagined movements of the affected vs. the unaffected hand.

TE

225

dynamic deconvolution method (Gitelman, Penny, Ashburner, & Friston, 2003). This PPI regressor, alongside the task regressors and the time course of the PFC seed region, were included in the statistical model. To remove any artifactual signal changes due to head motion, we also included 6 parameters describing the headmovements (three translations, three rotations) as confounds in the model. Linear contrasts pertaining to the main effects of the factorial design constituted the data for the second-stage analysis, which treated participants as a random factor. In this second-stage analysis, we carried out two analyses: (1) analysis of functional connectivity with the PFC (as parameterized by the regressor describing the time course of the PFC seed region) and (2) analysis of effective connectivity with the PFC as a function of affected vs. unaffected hand (as parameterized by the PPI regressor). We report the results of random effects analyses, using the family-wise error (FWE) correction method to control the false alarm rate (Friston et al., 1995). For our analysis of functional connectivity, we used a relatively stringent voxel-based statistical threshold (voxel threshold: pFWE = 0.05) and cluster threshold (k > 10), in order to limit spurious correlations that may be the result of general co-fluctuations during the scanning session. For analyses of effective connectivity, we used the more sensitive cluster-based correction for multiple comparisons (Friston, Holmes, Poline, Price, & Frith, 1996). For these analyses, we corrected for multiple comparisons at the cluster level (p < 0.05), based on a voxel level threshold of p < 0.01. We also tested for effective connectivity changes within a predefined set of regions that has previously been identified to be related to motor imagery, comprising the bilateral intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and dorsal premotor cortex (PMd). Within this limited search space, we corrected for multiple comparisons within 6 mm spheres surrounding previously identified peak maxima (see de Lange et al., 2005; de Lange, Helmich, & Toni, 2006; de Lange et al., 2007).

200 201

3

Q4 Fig. 1. Regions showing functional coupling with the dlPFC. Anatomical localization of regions showing significant positive coupling with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC). Green lines connect regions that exhibit significant functional coupling with the seed region, placed in the dlPFC (in light yellow). Renderings are thresholded at T = 13, for display purposes. (a) Axial rendering (top view), showing supplementary motor and pre- and postcentral clusters. (b) Coronal rendering (front view). (a) Sagittal rendering (lateral view). (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)

Please cite this article in press as: de Lange, F. P., et al. Altered connectivity between prefrontal and sensorimotor cortex in conversion paralysis. Neuropsychologia (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.029

247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262

264

266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277

279

281 282 283 284 285 286

ARTICLE IN PRESS

G Model NSY 3597 1–7 4

F.P. de Lange et al. / Neuropsychologia xxx (2010) xxx–xxx

Table 1 Functional and effective connectivity with dlPFC – anatomical localization. All reported coordinates are in MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) space. Stereotactic coordinates denote the peak of the clusters surviving correction for multiple comparisons. T-value

Supplementary motor area Supramarginal gyrus

Functional coupling

Central sulcus Increased positive coupling

Dorsal premotor cortex

Increased negative coupling

Postcentral gyrus Hippocampus

78.1 40.3 24.0 31.5

46 32 12 16

5.2

23

16.5 6.9 8.4

343 239 52

Corrected for multiple comparisons in an a priori defined search space.

296

4. Discussion

297

316

Previously, we investigated cerebral activity in 8 patients with a unilateral conversion paralysis during motor simulation of actions with their affected and unaffected hand. While motor imagery recruited comparable cerebral resources in the motor system, CP patients recruited additional cerebral resources in several clusters of the prefrontal cortex during motor imagery of the affected hand (de Lange et al., 2007). In the current study, we build on these findings and examined the functional and effective connectivity profile of the prefrontal cortex during motor imagery. We observed that different clusters within the prefrontal cortex had distinct functional connectivity patterns. While medial prefrontal clusters were functionally connected to inferior temporal regions, only the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) was functionally connected to the motor system. Moreover, the coupling between this region and various parts of the sensorimotor system was modulated by whether subjects imagined movements of the affected or the unaffected limb: dlPFC showed a stronger positive coupling with the dorsal premotor cortex (PMd), and a stronger negative coupling with the primary somatosensory cortex (S1). Below we will detail and interpret these findings.

317

4.1. dlPFC is functionally coupled with the sensorimotor system

318

In our previous study, we found differential responses during imagery of the affected and non-affected hand in a large prefrontal cluster, spanning both ventromedial and dorsolateral aspects of the superior frontal gyrus. In other words, this cluster incorporated responses from anatomically distinct regions (Cavada, Company, Tejedor, Cruz-Rizzolo, & Reinoso-Suarez, 2000; Chiavaras & Petrides, 2000; Petrides, 2005). Here we separately assessed the functional connectivity of each of the three local maxima of this large cluster. The maxima were localized in ventromedial (vmPFC), dorsomedial (dmPFC) and dorsolateral (dlPFC) prefrontal cortex, close to the location of functional differences reported in previous neuroimaging studies on CP patients (Cojan, Waber, Carruzzo, & Vuilleumier, 2009; Halligan et al., 2000; Marshall et al., 1997; Spence et al., 2000). The dlPFC showed distinct functional connectivity patterns from the medial clusters. Notably,

291 292 293 294

298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315

319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332

CT

290

RE

289

UN CO R

288

<0.001 <0.001 0.006 0.001 0.038a 0.002 0.022 0.014

Stereotactic coordinates x

y

−10 −62 −58 52

8 −18 −30 −16

z 48 28 34 42

24

2

70

18 −18 16

−42 −38 −16

52 50 −16

the dlPFC exhibited significant co-fluctuations with parietal, primary somatosensory, motor and supplementary motor areas (Fig. 1). In contrast, vmPFC and dmPFC clusters were not connected to the motor system, but instead to inferior temporal and contralateral dmPFC clusters. Taken together, this pattern of functional connectivity suggests that although these PFC clusters exibit similar local responses during motor imagery of the affected hand, they differ in terms of the long-range couplings to these local responses. More Q2 precisely, the dlPFC is coupled with parts of the motor system, in line with its presumed role in the selection of action plans (Frith et al., 2000b; Passingham, 1993; Rowe et al., 2007) (Fig. 2).

333

4.2. Increased positive coupling between dlPFC and PMd during imagery of the affected hand

344

Analysis of effective connectivity showed a larger positive coupling between dlPFC and PMd during motor imagery of the affected hand. This means that a unitary increase in dlPFC activity when CP patients were imagining movements of the affected hand resulted in significantly greater activity in the PMd, as compared to trials where the patients imagined movements of the non-affected hand. PMd is one of the two core regions that are specifically involved in mental simulation of hand movements (de Lange et al., 2005). The activity of this region is likely to be related to the generation of motor plans. For example, prolonged electrical stimulation of the macaque’s precentral gyrus evokes complex upper-limb movements (Graziano, Taylor, & Moore, 2002), and premotor neurons are known to select and encode various movement parameters. The increased coupling between dlPFC and PMd during motor imagery of the affected hand may be a cerebral counterpart of the increased attention to action that CP patients deploy during the generation of action plans of the paralyzed limb (Rowe, Friston, Frackowiak, & Passingham, 2002). Given the matched motor imagery performance of these CP patients during trials involving either hand (de Lange et al., 2007), it is conceivable that the increase in dlPFC-premotor connectivity reflects a compensatory mechanism, namely an increased prefrontal drive towards premotor regions supporting the imagery process. Future studies might be able to test the directionality of the change in prefrontal-premotor connectivity, as predicted by this hypothesis.

346

4.3. Increased negative coupling between dlPFC and S1 during imagery of the affected hand

371

The dlPFC had larger negative couplings with the left and right postcentral gyrus, as well as with the hippocampus. The postcentral clusters fell within cyto-architectonic boundaries of primary somatosensory cortices (S1). We interpret our findings as stronger negative coupling during imagery of the affected hand, rather than stronger positive coupling during imagery of the unaffected hand,

373

ED

295

There was also increased negative coupling between dlPFC and the right hippocampus. Activity in the postcentral clusters was overall suppressed during motor imagery (left postcentral gyrus: T = 6.27, pFWE = 0.035; right postcentral gyrus: T = 9.72, pFWE < 0.001), in line with previous studies (Cheron & Borenstein, 1992; Jahn et al., 2004). Therefore, the current set of results suggest that activity in dlPFC and suppression of postcentral regions are coupled more strongly during imagery of the affected hand.

287

Corrected p-value

PR

a

Cluster size

F

Region

OO

Contrast

Please cite this article in press as: de Lange, F. P., et al. Altered connectivity between prefrontal and sensorimotor cortex in conversion paralysis. Neuropsychologia (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.029

334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343

345

347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370

372

374 375 376 377 378

G Model NSY 3597 1–7

ARTICLE IN PRESS 5

OO

F

F.P. de Lange et al. / Neuropsychologia xxx (2010) xxx–xxx

407

4.4. Coupling between dlPFC and the hippocampus

408

We also observed larger negative coupling between dlPFC and the right hippocampus during motor imagery of the affected hand. The hippocampus is known to directly interact with the prefrontal cortex during the formation and retrieval of associative memory traces (Dolan & Fletcher, 1997; Gaffan, 2005; Schacter & Wagner, 1999). More specifically, the control of unwanted memories is associated with increased dlPFC activation and reduced hippocampal activation (Anderson et al., 2004). We speculate that the negative coupling between dlPFC and hippocampus observed during imagery of movements involving the affected hand could mediate a control mechanism to decouple (imagined) movements of the affected hand from aversive memories associated with those movements. It remains to be seen whether remission of CP symp-

384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405

409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420

CT

383

RE

382

UN CO R

381

toms is supported by increased efficiency of this putative control mechanism.

421

4.5. vmPFC and dlPFC: self-monitoring and action selection?

423

ED

406

in view of S1 showing strong task-related suppression of activity. Suppression of somatosensory cortex during motor imagery has been previously observed (Jahn et al., 2004; Rossini et al., 1996). Current models of motor control assert that during selfproduced actions, a prediction of the sensory consequences of the action is created, which is subtracted from the observed sensory afferent information (Shadmehr & Krakauer, 2008; Wolpert & Ghahramani, 2000). This results in attenuated somatosensory perception, even when motor plans are generated but not executed (Voss, Ingram, Wolpert, & Haggard, 2008) or merely imagined (Cheron & Borenstein, 1992; Grush, 2004). The stronger negative coupling between dlPFC and S1 may therefore be directly related to the stronger positive coupling between dlPFC and PMd. When motor plans are generated (in PMd), sensory consequences are simultaneously computed, leading to sensory attenuation (in S1). In line with this, we observed a stronger modulatory influence of dlPFC during the generation of action plans of the affected limb that was positive in sign for the PMd and negative in sign for S1. The stronger inhibitory coupling between dlPFC and S1 during motor imagery of the affected hand could also provide an explanation for the decreased excitability of the primary motor cortex that has previously been observed in CP (Liepert et al., 2009). Activity in S1 can strongly contribute to the excitability of the motor system as measured by motor evoked potentials (Avenanti, Bolognini, Maravita, & Aglioti, 2007). Our results suggest that this reduced excitability may be the result of larger dlPFC activity during generation of action plans of the affected hand.

379 380

PR

Fig. 2. Regions showing effective coupling with the dlPFC. Anatomical localization of regions showing significant positive (in red) and negative (in blue) effective coupling with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC). Green lines connect regions that exhibit significant functional coupling with the seed region, placed in the dlPFC (in yellow). Red color indicates positive coupling, blue color indicates negative coupling. Renderings are thresholded at T = 3, for display purposes. (a) Axial rendering, view from top, showing precentral and postcentral clusters. (b) Coronal rendering (back view), showing postcentral clusters. (a) Axial rendering, view from bottom, showing right hippocampus. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)

422

While previous studies have often interpreted changes in the prefrontal activation profile of CP in a unitary fashion, our current set of results, alongside anatomical and functional considerations, suggest that alterations in different nodes of the prefrontal cortex may be linked to distinct aspects of this psychopathological condition. While vmPFC showed functional connectivity with inferior temporal cortices, there was no functional or effective connectivity with the sensorimotor system. The vmPFC has close connections with limbic areas and it is involved in emotion regulation (Ochsner & Gross, 2005) as well as interoceptive attention and self-referential and autobiographical processing (Amodio & Frith, 2006; Gilbert et al., 2006; Summerfield, Hassabis, & Maguire, 2009). Accordingly, we have previously attributed the vmPFC activity differences to heightened self-monitoring during motor imagery of the affected limb. The current set of results extend these observations by showing that vmPFC does not directly impinge on the sensorimotor system. Conversely, the dlPFC showed extensive functional connectivity with the sensorimotor system, and altered its connectivity as a function of whether CP patients engaged in motor imagery of the affected or unaffected hand. Therefore, dlPFC may be more directly involved in mediating the altered sensory and motor symptoms observed in CP. It might be relevant to test whether experimental manipulation of dlPFC activity (as achievable with transcranial magnetic stimulation, for instance) could lead to a modulation of the sensorimotor symptoms of CP patients. Therefore, we propose that both mechanisms (heightened self-monitoring and action selection) have a role in conversion paralysis, but subserved by anatomically distinct parts of the prefrontal cortex.

424

5. Conclusion

453

Our results indicate that there are altered connectivity patterns between the dlPFC and various sensorimotor nodes during the formation of action plans of the affected arm. There is increased positive coupling between dlPFC and PMd, while there is increased negative coupling between dlPFC and S1 and the hippocampus. These results provide a link between previous reports of both heightened prefrontal and reduced sensorimotor activity in CP.

454

Please cite this article in press as: de Lange, F. P., et al. Altered connectivity between prefrontal and sensorimotor cortex in conversion paralysis. Neuropsychologia (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.029

425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452

455 456 457 458 459 460

G Model NSY 3597 1–7

ARTICLE IN PRESS F.P. de Lange et al. / Neuropsychologia xxx (2010) xxx–xxx

463

Competing interests

464

The authors have no competing interests.

465

Acknowledgments

466

This study was supported by the Netherlands Science Foundation (NWO: grant number 446-07-003 awarded to FdL; 452-03-339 awarded to IT and 451-02-115 awarded to KR) and the Dutch Brain Foundation (Hersenstichting Nederland, grant number 12F04(2).19 awarded to KR and FdL).

467 468 469 470

References

472

Abbruzzese, G., Trompetto, C., & Schieppati, M. (1996). The excitability of the human motor cortex increases during execution and mental imagination of sequential but not repetitive finger movements. Experimental Brain Research, 111(3), 465–472. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders DSM-IV. American Psychiatric Association. Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(4), 268–277. Anderson, M. C., Ochsner, K. N., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J., Robertson, E., Gabrieli, S. W., et al. (2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science, 303(5655), 232–235. Avenanti, A., Bolognini, N., Maravita, A., & Aglioti, S. M. (2007). Somatic and motor components of action simulation. Current Biology, 17(24), 2129–2135. Burgmer, M., Konrad, C., Jansen, A., Kugel, H., Sommer, J., Heindel, W., et al. (2006). Abnormal brain activation during movement observation in patients with conversion paralysis. Neuroimage, 29(4), 1336–1343. Caspers, S., Geyer, S., Schleicher, A., Mohlberg, H., Amunts, K., & Zilles, K. (2006). The human inferior parietal cortex: Cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. Neuroimage, 33(2), 430–448. Castelli, F., Happe, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: A functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage, 12(3), 314–325. Cavada, C., Company, T., Tejedor, J., Cruz-Rizzolo, R. J., & Reinoso-Suarez, F. (2000). The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. A review. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 220–242. Cheron, G., & Borenstein, S. (1992). Mental movement simulation affects the N30 frontal component of the somatosensory evoked potential. Electroencephalography Clinical Neurophysiology, 84(3), 288–292. Chiavaras, M. M., & Petrides, M. (2000). Orbitofrontal sulci of the human and macaque monkey brain. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 422(1), 35–54. Cojan, Y., Waber, L., Carruzzo, A., & Vuilleumier, P. (2009). Motor inhibition in hysterical conversion paralysis. NeuroImage, de Lange, Floris P., Hagoort, P., & Toni, I. (2005). Neural topography and content of movement representations. The Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(1), 97–112. de Lange, F. P., Helmich, R. C., & Toni, I. (2006). Posture influences motor imagery: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 33(2), 609–617. de Lange, F. P., Roelofs, K., & Toni, I. (2007). Increased self-monitoring during imagined movements in conversion paralysis. Neuropsychologia, 45(9), 2051–2058. de Lange, Floris P., Roelofs, K., & Toni, I. (2008). Motor imagery: A window into the mechanisms and alterations of the motor system. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 44(5), 494–506. Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., Durozard, D., & Baverel, G. (1993). Central activation of autonomic effectors during mental simulation of motor actions in man. Journal of Physiology, 461, 549–563. Dolan, R. J., & Fletcher, P. C. (1997). Dissociating prefrontal and hippocampal function in episodic memory encoding. Nature, 388(6642), 582–585. Duvernoy, Henri M., Cabanis, E. A., & Vannson, J. L. (1991). The human brain: Surface, three-dimensional sectional anatomy and MRI. Wien: Springer-Verlag. Eickhoff, S. B., Stephan, K. E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G. R., Amunts, K., et al. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage, 25(4), 1325–1335. Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Worsley, K. J., Poline, J. B., Frith, C., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1995). Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach. Human Brain Mapping, 2, 189–210. Friston, K. J., Holmes, A., Poline, J. B., Price, C. J., & Frith, C. D. (1996). Detecting Activations in PET and fMRI: Levels of Inference and Power. Neuroimage, 4(3), 223–235. Friston, K. J., Buechel, C., Fink, G. R., Morris, J., Rolls, E., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage, 6(3), 218–229. Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. J., & Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences, 355(1404), 1771–1788.

476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534

CT

475

RE

474

UN CO R

473

ED

471

Frith, C. D., Moody, S. L., & Driver, J. (2000). The role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the selection of action control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII. Cambridge: MIT Press. Fuster, J. M. (2000). Executive frontal functions. Experimental Brain Research, 133(1), 66–70. Gaffan, D. (2005). Neuroscience. Widespread cortical networks underlie memory and attention. Science, 309(5744), 2172–2173. Geyer, S., Schleicher, A., & Zilles, K. (1999). Areas 3a, 3b, and 1 of human primary somatosensory cortex. Neuroimage, 10(1), 63–83. Ghaffar, O., Staines D W. R., & Feinstein, A. (2006). Unexplained neurologic symptoms: An fMRI study of sensory conversion disorder. Neurology, 67(11), 2036–2038. Gilbert, S. J., Spengler, S., Simons, J. S., Steele, J. D., Lawrie, S. M., Frith, C. D., et al. (2006). Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): A meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(6), 932–948. Gitelman, Darren R., Penny, William D., Ashburner, J., & Friston, Karl J. (2003). Modeling regional and psychophysiologic interactions in fMRI: The importance of hemodynamic deconvolution. Neuroimage, 19(1), 200–207. Goldberg, I. I., Harel, M., & Malach, R. (2006). When the brain loses its self: Prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron, 50(2), 329–339. Graziano, M. S., Taylor, C. S., & Moore, T. (2002). Complex movements evoked by microstimulation of precentral cortex. Neuron, 34(5), 841–851. Grefkes, C., Geyer, S., Schormann, T., Roland, P., & Zilles, K. (2001). Human somatosensory area 2: Observer-independent cytoarchitectonic mapping, interindividual variability, and population map. Neuroimage, 14(3), 617–631. Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(3), 377–396. Halligan, P. W., Athwal, B. S., Oakley, D. A., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (2000). Imaging hypnotic paralysis: Implications for conversion hysteria. The lancet, 355(9208), 986–987. Halligan, Peter W., Bass, C., & Marshall, John C. (2001). Contemporary approaches to the study of hysteria: Clinical and theoretical perspectives. USA: Oxford University Press. Jahn, K., Deutschlander, A., Stephan, T., Strupp, M., Wiesmann, M., & Brandt, T. (2004). Brain activation patterns during imagined stance and locomotion in functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage, 22(4), 1722–1731. Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behavioral and Brain Science, 17, 187–245. Kanaan, R. A. A., Craig, T. K. J., Wessely, S. C., & David, A. S. (2007). Imaging repressed memories in motor conversion disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(2), 202–1202. Liepert, J., Hassa, T., Tüscher, O., & Schmidt, R. (2009). Abnormal motor Q3 excitability in patients with psychogenic paresis. Journal of Neurology, 256(1), 121–126. Mailis-Gagnon, A., Giannoylis, I., Downar, J., Kwan, C. L., Mikulis, D. J., Crawley, A. P., et al. (2003). Altered central somatosensory processing in chronic pain patients with “hysterical” anesthesia. Neurology, 60(9), 1501–1507. Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W., Fink, G. R., Wade, D. T., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1997). The functional anatomy of a hysterical paralysis. Cognition, 64(1), B1–B8. Maruff, P., & Velakoulis, D. (2000). The voluntary control of motor imagery. Imagined movements in individuals with feigned motor impairment and conversion disorder. Neuropsychologia, 38(9), 1251–1260. Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9(5), 242–249. Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformation of one’s body. Journal of Experimemenal Psychology General, 116(2), 172–191. Parsons, L. M., Gabrieli, J. D., Phelps, E. A., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1998). Cerebrally lateralized mental representations of hand shape and movement. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(16), 6539–6548. Passingham, R. E. (1993). The frontal lobes and voluntary action. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Petersson, K. M., Nichols, T. E., Poline, J. B., & Holmes, A. P. (1999). Statistical limitations in functional neuroimaging. II. Signal detection and statistical inference. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 354, 1261–1281. Petrides, M. (2005). Lateral prefrontal cortex: Architectonic and functional organization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences, 360(1456), 781–795. Price, C. J., & Friston, K. J. (2002). Functional imaging studies of neuropsychological patients: Applications and limitations. Neurocase, 8(5), 345–354. Rajkowska, G., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1995). Cytoarchitectonic definition of prefrontal areas in the normal human cortex: II. Variability in locations of areas 9 and 46 and relationship to the Talairach Coordinate System. Cerebral Cortex, 5, 323–337. Roelofs, K., & Spinhoven, P. (2007). Trauma and medically unexplained symptoms towards an integration of cognitive and neuro-biological accounts. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 798–820. Roelofs, K., Näring, G. W. B., Keijsers, G. P. J., Hoogduin, C. A. L., Van Galen, G. P., & Maris, E. (2001). Motor imagery in conversion paralysis. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 6(1), 21–40. Rossini, P. M., Caramia, D., Bassetti, M. A., Pasqualetti, P., Tecchio, F., & Bernardi, G. (1996). Somatosensory evoked potentials during the ideation and execution of individual finger movements. Muscle Nerve, 19, 191–202. Rowe, J., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R., & Passingham, R. (2002). Attention to action: Specific modulation of corticocortical interactions in humans. Neuroimage, 17(2), 988–998.

F

These results suggest that reduced motor responsitivity may be linked to altered dorsolateral prefrontal-motor connectivity.

OO

462

461

PR

6

Please cite this article in press as: de Lange, F. P., et al. Altered connectivity between prefrontal and sensorimotor cortex in conversion paralysis. Neuropsychologia (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.029

535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620

G Model NSY 3597 1–7

ARTICLE IN PRESS F.P. de Lange et al. / Neuropsychologia xxx (2010) xxx–xxx

629 630 631 632 633

F

628

OO

627

PR

626

ED

625

CT

624

Stone, J., Zeman, A., Simonotto, E., Meyer, M., Azuma, R., Flett, S., et al. (2007). FMRI in patients with motor conversion symptoms and controls with simulated weakness. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(9), 961–1961. Summerfield, J. J., Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2009). Cortical midline involvement in autobiographical memory. NeuroImage, 44, 1188–1200. Voss, M., Ingram, J. N., Wolpert, D. M., & Haggard, P. (2008). Mere expectation to move causes attenuation of sensory signals. PLoS ONE, 3(8), e2866. Vuilleumier, Patrik. (2005). Hysterical conversion and brain function. Progress in Brain Research, 150, 309–329. Vuilleumier, P., Chicherio, C., Assal, F., Schwartz, S., Slosman, D., & Landis, T. (2001). Functional neuroanatomical correlates of hysterical sensorimotor loss. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 124(Pt. 6), 1077–1090. Wolpert, D. M., & Ghahramani, Z. (2000). Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1212–1217.

RE

623

Rowe, J. B., Sakai, K., Lund, T. E., Ramsoy, T., Christensen, M. S., Baare, W. F., et al. (2007). Is the prefrontal cortex necessary for establishing cognitive sets? Journal of Neuroscience, 27(48), 13303–13310. Schacter, D. L., & Wagner, A. D. (1999). Medial temporal lobe activations in fMRI and PET studies of episodic encoding and retrieval. Hippocampus, 9(1), 7–24. Sekiyama, K. (1982). Kinesthetic aspects of mental representations in the identification of left and right hands. Perception and Psychophysics, 32(2), 89–95. Shadmehr, R., & Krakauer, J. W. (2008). A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Experimental Brain Research, 185(3), 359–381. Spence, S. A., Crimlisk, H. L., Cope, H., Ron, M. A., & Grasby, P. M. (2000). Discrete neurophysiological correlates in prefrontal cortex during hysterical and feigned disorder of movement. The Lancet, 355(9211), 1243–1244.

UN CO R

621 622

7

Please cite this article in press as: de Lange, F. P., et al. Altered connectivity between prefrontal and sensorimotor cortex in conversion paralysis. Neuropsychologia (2010), doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.029

634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642 643 644 645 646 647

Altered connectivity between prefrontal and ...

disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). .... The stimuli were presented using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral systems,. 153 ...... Cavada, C., Company, T., Tejedor, J., Cruz-Rizzolo, R. J., & Reinoso-Suarez, F. (2000). 494.

947KB Sizes 2 Downloads 262 Views

Recommend Documents

Relationship between drug use and prefrontal
8. Fontaine A, Azouvi P, Remy P, Bussel B, Samson. Y. Functional anatomy of neuropsychological deficits after severe traumatic brain injury. Neurol- ogy 1999;10:1963 – 8. 9. Corrigan JD, Rust E, Lamb-Hart G. The nature and extent of substance abuse

Relationship between drug use and prefrontal ...
scores of 39.2 (A), 35.0 (D), 35.1 (E) and. 109.3 (total). Non-smokers (n = 48) had scores. Table 1. Partial correlations between FrSBe subscales and parameters ...

Corridors and connectivity
George Street, Brisbane 4001, Australia; *Author for correspondence (e-mail: [email protected]). Received 22 ... a means for detecting gene flow, direct methods. (i.e. trapping) are ..... (Compliance no. ISO 11794, Veterinary Marketing.

Ventromedial and Orbital Prefrontal Neurons ...
Jun 23, 2010 - case both when we go shopping simply because we are hungry ..... E, OFC neuron activated just before feedback in self-initiated trials with.

Connectivity
When you're on the road, you can stay connected with a 3G connectivity package from. Verizon Wireless. Chromebook that have built-in 3G include up to 100MB ...

Connectivity
Free data available for 2 years from the time you first activate your 3G service. 2. A day pass offers unlimited data access for 24 hours from the time of data purchase. 3. Any purchase of additional data expires after 30 days from the date of data p

Ventromedial and Orbital Prefrontal Neurons ...
Jun 23, 2010 - neurons encoding action increases (red line), with the biggest change .... Ongur D, Price JL (2000) The organization of networks within the orbital ... roles for cingulate and orbitofrontal cortex in decisions and social behav-.

Connectivity
Broadband service for 2 years, provided by Verizon Wireless. Also available are an unlimited day pass for just $9.99 and pay-as-you-go rates that are.

Increased Automaticity and Altered Temporal Preparation Following ...
5.5 ± 0.7 39.2 ± 2.4. Mean RT (ms). 262 ± 6. 279 ± 6. 278 ± 4. 480 ± 24. Std Deviation of RT (ms). 70 ± 7. 81 ± 7. 95 ± 11. 569 ± 79. Number of false alarms.

Effective connectivity: Influence, causality and ...
Available online xxxx. Keywords: .... whether some causal link is likely to be present (by comparing models with and ...... This simple model has proven to be a useful tool in many fields, including ...... Human Brain Mapping annual Meeting.

Consensus, cohesion and connectivity
Jun 23, 2017 - ity increases the predictive power of social influence theory, shown by re-using experimental data ... sciences—social cohesion (Section 4)—that was defined consider- ing a multiplicity of independent ..... but in actuality there a

Activity in Inferior Parietal and Medial Prefrontal ... - Caltech Authors
Jan 31, 2013 - The activation of past outcomes in memory is evoked as a possible mechanism to ... to increase with the probability of an upcoming punishment.

prefrontal system dysfunction and credit card debt
variables (age, sex, education, and income), credit card debt was asso- ciated with the .... cate prefrontal systems in cognition and behavior regarding finances.

Function and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex
2MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge CB2 7EF, UK. We propose that ... Published online 2 April 2007. One contribution of 14 to a ... compared with other animals limits the degree to which one might ... cognitive functions of the brain ar

An integrate and fire model of prefrontal cortex ...
[email protected]. November 23, 2005. Running title: Prefrontal cortex model. 1 ...... test of the evaluation software that they used to assess task related activity.

Prefrontal cortex dysfunction and attitudes toward money
b Department of Economics and International Business, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA 19104, ... Keywords: Neuroeconomics; Credit cards; Money attitudes.

Prefrontal substrates of empathy: Psychometric ...
between prefrontal system dysfunction and empathy, particularly emotional empathy and .... factor analytic data support the validity of the three scales. (Grace et ...

Prefrontal substrates of empathy: Psychometric ...
an important component of adaptive interpersonal relation- ships, and necessary for effective parenting and ... Cognitive components of empathy are akin to ''theory of mind,'' or the ability to mentally represent other ... during the Prisoner's Dilem

Unconscious errors enhance prefrontal-occipital ... - Semantic Scholar
Nov 24, 2009 - synchrony was taken from three temporal windows (first averaging from 2–12 Hz): −1200 to −300 ms ... the analyses using different time windows did not appreciably alter the results. Averaged data were entered .... unconscious err

Altered collagen metabolism and delayed healing in ...
an inability of the tissue to maintain appropriate levels of collagen in this inflammatory wound environment. PMID: 17173649 [PubMed - in process]. Wound Repair Regen. 1995 Apr;3(2):204-12. Related Articles, Links. Page 1 of 1. Entrez PubMed. 1/5/200

Development of rostral prefrontal cortex and cognitive ...
Jan 10, 2008 - to functional neuroimaging in adults, and put forward in relation to .... Öngür et al.8 proposed that a 'medial' network ...... and social cognition.

Architecture and dynamics of the primate prefrontal ...
... in the prefrontal cortex by means of computer simulations of the dynamics of a model prefrontal cortical circuit. .... 1998) suggest that pyramidal cells in the PFC area related ..... Time courses of the synaptic inputs to the Pd cell of column #

Face Recognition in Surgically Altered Faces Using Optimization and ...
translation and scale invariance [3]. Russell C.Eberhart (2006) emphasis that the ... Each particle in the search space evolves its candidate solution over time, making use of its individual memory and knowledge gained by the swarm as a ... exchange

Activity in Inferior Parietal and Medial Prefrontal ... - Caltech Authors
Jan 31, 2013 - The activation of past outcomes in memory is evoked as a possible mechanism to ... to increase with the probability of an upcoming punishment.