Schizophrenia Research 70 (2004) 81 – 89 www.elsevier.com/locate/schres

Co-administration of atypical antipsychotics and antidepressants disturbs contrast detection in schizophrenia S. Sheremata a,c, Y. Chen b,c,* a Department of Psychology, Boston University, USA Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, USA c Room 123D, Mailman Research Center, McLean Hospital, 115 Mill Street, Room 123D, Belmont, MA 02478, USA b

Received 23 June 2003; received in revised form 10 September 2003; accepted 12 September 2003 Available online 27 November 2003

Abstract Atypical antipsychotics (APDs) antagonize both serotonin and dopamine receptors. This antagonism is, however, often confounded by co-administration of other medications, such as antidepressants, that affect pharmacological activity at these receptors. While it is known that the modulation of dopamine affects cognitive processes such as working memory, the interactions between APDs and antidepressants in behaviors including sensory processes are not clear. In this study, we investigated the effect of combined treatment with antidepressants and atypical antipsychotics on memory-related visual processing in schizophrenia. We employed (1) contrast detection, a task requiring the maintenance of visual signals over a short period of time; and (2) direction discrimination, a task not requiring maintenance of visual signals. On contrast detection, the performance was significantly worse in the patients taking both APDs and antidepressants than in patients taking just APDs. On direction discrimination, however, the performance did not differ between the patients tasking just APD and those taking both APDs and antidepressants. Given that antidepressants interfere with APD’s stimulation of D1 receptors via agonism of serotonin receptors, the poor performance on contrast detection suggests that the interaction between these two types of psychotropic drugs selectively disrupts the sensory processes requiring retention of visual information. D 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Antidepressant; Atypical antipsychotics; Visual processing; Working memory; Schizophrenia

1. Introduction Polypharmacy is common in the treatment of schizophrenia. Often this polypharmacy is the result of secondary symptoms such as depressed mood. Indeed even considering the efficacy of medications * Corresponding author. Room 123D, Mailman Research Center, McLean Hospital, 115 Mill Street, Room 123D, Belmont, MA 02478, USA. Tel.: +1-617-855-3615; fax: +1-617-855-2778. E-mail address: [email protected] (Y. Chen). 0920-9964/$ - see front matter D 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.005

such as atypical antipsychotics, depressed mood remains a common problem (Siris, 2000). Recent animal studies suggest that the modulation of serotonin via antidepressant drugs such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) interferes with the action of dopamine-serotonin interactions of atypical antipsychotics (APDs) (Ichikawa et al., 2001b; Westerink et al., 2001). In schizophrenic patients, cognitive performance was improved when a 5HT2a antagonist was added to typical antipsychotic treatment (Poyurovsky et al., 2003).

82

S. Sheremata, Y. Chen / Schizophrenia Research 70 (2004) 81–89

Animal research has revealed how APDs modulate different subtypes of dopamine and serotonin neurotransmitters. Several studies have found that APDs indirectly stimulate D1 receptors in the medial prefrontal cortex (Moghaddam and Bunney, 1990; Nomikos et al., 1994; Pehek and Yamamoto, 1994; Kuroki et al., 1999; Volonte´ et al., 1997; Rollema et al., 2000; Ichikawa et al., 2001a; Westerink et al., 2001). This indirect stimulation occurs through direct stimulation of 5HT1a -receptors by combined 5HT2a and D2 antagonism (Ichikawa et al., 2001a,b). This interaction may be illustrated as following: 5HT2a  D2 ! 5HT1a z ! D1 z It is important to point out here that neither D2 nor 5HT2a antagonism alone is sufficient to stimulate 5HT1a receptors, a precursor of D1 receptor stimulation; rather it is the combined antagonism of D2 and 5HT2a that indirectly results in the stimulation of D1 receptors (Kuroki et al., 1999; Rollema et al., 2000; Ichikawa et al., 2001a; Westerink et al., 2001). The administration of 5HT2a agonists has been shown to block the stimulation of D1 receptors by APDs (Rollema et al., 2000; Ichikawa et al., 2001b; Westerink et al., 2001). Specifically, DOI, a 5HT2a/2c receptor agonist, attenuates stimulation of prefrontal dopamine receptors by clozapine (Ichikawa et al., 2001a). Many antidepressants such as SSRIs are 5HT2a agonists. Thus, in principle, combined treatment of these antidepressants and APDs in patients may interfere with APDs’ stimulation of D1 receptors. The interaction of antidepressants and APDs may be demonstrated as: APD Antidepressant Combined effect

 5HT2a – D2 ! 5HT1az ! D1z + 5HT2a  D2 ! 5HT1az ! D1z (weakened to diminished stimulation)

That is to say, co-administration of antidepressants and APDs can decrease the ability of APDs to stimulate D1 receptors because of a canceling effect of antidepressants on the APDs’ antagonism of 5HT2a. In order to determine behavioral consequences of this co-administration of psychtropic drugs, we can examine visual and cognitive responses that are related to D1 modulation.

Evidence suggests that D1 receptor stimulation has a principal role in memory dependent visual tasks. For example, injection of D1 antagonists, SCH23390 and SCH39166, in prefrontal areas of monkeys induced errors and increase latency during performance of working memory tasks at delays as short as 1.5 s (e.g. Sawaguchi and Goldman-Rakic, 1994). Assuming that APDs ameliorate cortical hypo-dopaminergia in schizophrenia, the patients taking APDs should have improved or normalized performance on visual perception tasks requiring maintenance of visual signals. If this modulation of dopamine by APDs were blocked by the co-administration of antidepressants, then performance would be again impaired. This study examines the effects of dopaminergic– serotonergic interaction on visual tasks that differ in memory requirements by testing schizophrenia patients taking both antidepressants and APDs and those taking only APDs. If antagonism of both 5HT2a and D2 receptors were necessary for stimulating 5HT1a and then D1 receptors, the combination of antidepressants and APDs would reduce the extent to which D1 receptors are stimulated. Because D1 receptor stimulation is important for working memory related tasks, patients taking APDs and antidepressants would be expected to perform worse on a task that requires maintenance of a visual signal, but perform similarly on a task that does not has such a requirement. Finally, not all combinations of antidepressant and APD medications are expected to affect performance equally on memory-related tasks. Stimulation of D1 has been shown to depend on a relation of low D2 antagonism relative to 5HT2a antagonism. This relationship between D2 and 5HT2a antagonism suggests that each APD stimulates D1 receptors to different extents, depending on its relative potency as a D2 antagonist compared to its potency as a 5HT2a antagonist. For example, Rollema et al. (2000) found that at equal doses, clozapine, a relatively weak D2 as compared with 5HT2a antagonist, leads to greater stimulation of D1 receptors than does ziprasidone, which is a more potent D2 receptor antagonist. In addition, certain antidepressants exert effects not only on serotonin, but also on dopamine and norepinephrine. For these reasons, this study included only those subjects taking clozapine, olanzapine, or quetiapine

S. Sheremata, Y. Chen / Schizophrenia Research 70 (2004) 81–89

83

Patients were discharged during the past year from a psychiatric hospital and met DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Patients were independently diagnosed before admittance into the study based upon review of Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV (Spitzer et al., 1994) and evaluation of hospital records. Subjects in each group were matched for age, sex, education and socio-economic status. The verbal IQ scores between two subject groups are slightly, but not significantly, different ( p>0.1), partly because of the small sample sizes in each group. We do not expect, however, that the intelligence scores have a substantial effect on visual sensory responses; which of course needs to be confirmed in future studies using a larger sample. Lastly, patients in each medication group did not differ in brief psychotic rating score (BPRS) ( p>0.1) (Table 1).

for APDs, and trazadone, clomipramine or those in the class of SSRIs for antidepressants. In Experiment 1, subjects preformed a contrast detection task that requires comparing visual signals in two consecutive time intervals. Maintenance of visual signals across two time intervals is necessary for performing the task. In Experiment 2, subjects performed a direction discrimination task, in addition to the contrast detection task. The direction discrimination task requires that subjects respond each time when a stimulus was presented. Thus, no maintenance of visual signals is necessary for performing this task. 1.1. Experiment 1 1.1.1. Methods 1.1.1.1. Subjects. Thirty-two schizophrenia patients and seventeen normal controls participated in this experiment. Eighteen patients were taking APDs (mean chlorpromazine hydrochloride dose equivalent (CPZ): 454.5 mg (r = 245.7 mg)) and fourteen patients taking both APDs (CPZ: 643.5 mg (r = 277.3 mg) and antidepressants. Four patients in the co-administration group were excluded because of the types of antidepressants they took (see above on this page). The remaining patients in the co-administration group took one of three SSRIs, Celexa (n = 2), Prozac (n = 2) and Zoloft (n = 6). Medications were prescribed independently of this research study. None of the normal controls were taking medication at the time of the study.

1.1.1.2. Stimuli. The target was a sinusoidal vertical grating with a spatial frequency of 0.5 cycles per degree. The gratings were moving at 5 Hz (10j/s) horizontally either to the right or to the left. The target was generated in a Macintosh computer (Quadra 600), and was presented within a circular aperture of 10j of visual angle. 1.1.1.3. Procedure. Contrast detection thresholds were determined using a two-alternative forced choice using a staircase method (3 down/1 up f 79% correct). During each trial, each of two temporal intervals was signaled by a tone. The target appeared randomly at one interval across trials; dur-

Table 1 Demographic information of subject (Experiment 1); mean (standard deviation) Group

Sex

APD + antidepressant

7F, 3M

APD

NC

a b

BPRSa

Age

Education (years)

94 (11.0)

41.7 (7.4)

13.5 (1.5)

42 (14.7)

7F, 11M

105.3 (11.0)

35.0 (8.3)

14.6 (3.4)

37.9 (13.1)

13F, 4M

102.6 (11.3)

37.5 (8.8)

14.8 (2.8)

NA

BPRS—brief psychiatric rating scale. SES—socioeconomic status.

Verbal IQ

SESb I 10% II 30% III 60% I 33.3% II 33.3% III 27.8% V 6.6% I 17.6% II 35.3% III 47.1%

84

S. Sheremata, Y. Chen / Schizophrenia Research 70 (2004) 81–89

ing the alternate interval a blank screen with a luminance equal to the target was presented. Subject’s task was to indicate if the target came during the first or second interval. Their responses were given by pressing one of two designated keys on a computer keyboard. Each of the two intervals lasted 300 ms followed by an inter-trial interval of 450 ms. The level of contrast was determined by the staircase method, and was decreased by 5% of current level for each set of three consecutive correct responses, or increased by 5% for each incorrect response. Instruction and a practice session were provided prior to data collection. Experimenters were blind to diagnostic and medication condition of the subjects during testing. 1.1.1.4. Results. Contrast detection thresholds were significantly higher in schizophrenic patients taking both antidepressants and APDs (0.0047, r=.0019) than those of patients taking only APDs (0.0019, r = 0.0011) (t(25,1) = 4.26, p < 0.01) and than normal controls (0.0021, r = 0.0010) (t(25,1) = 4.01, p < 0.01). There was no significant group difference between patients taking only APD medication and normal controls (t(32,1) = 0.55, p>0.1) (Fig. 1). The threshold elevation in the patients taking both drugs was not related to their psychosis, as BPRS scores between the two patient groups did not differ

(t(26,1) = 0.76, p>0.1) and contrast detection thresholds were not correlated with BPRS scores (r = 0.16, p>0.1). It is unclear, however, the BPRS scores in the two patient groups were similar before the drugs were administered. In future studies, comparing contrast detection performance before and after the drug treatments may elicit the effect on a relation of behavioral responses (such as contrast detection) and psychotic status (measured by BPRS scores) in patients. The correlation between contrast detection threshold and dose of atypical antipsychotics, expressed as CPZ equivalents, was 0.24 ( p>0.05) in the patients taking APDs only. The contrast detection thresholds were 0.0042, 0.0061, and 0.0043 in the patients taking Celexa (n = 2), Prozac (n = 2) and Zoloft (n = 6), respectively. The poor performance on contrast detection, as indicated by high thresholds, may be attributed to two processes that are required for performing this task. First, encoding of contrast signals is required in order to detect the visual target (gratings). Second, maintenance of the visual signal over two time intervals is also required in order to indicate in which interval the target is present. To determine which process is primarily affected by the co-administration of antidepressant and APD drugs, we conducted another experiment, in which a visual direction discrimination task was used. This direction discrimination task is

Fig. 1. Group thresholds in contrast detection. Three subject groups are included—normal controls (NC) (n = 17), schizophrenic patients taking both atypical antipsychotic drugs (APD) and antidepressants (SZ (APD + antidepressant)) (n = 10), and schizophrenic patients taking just APD (SZ (APD)) (n = 18). The thresholds are represented on ordinate. High threshold values mean poor performance. The error bars indicate F 1 standard deviation.

S. Sheremata, Y. Chen / Schizophrenia Research 70 (2004) 81–89

85

Table 2 Demographic information of subject (Experiment 2); mean (standard deviation) Group

Sex

Verbal IQ

Age

Education (years)

BPRS

SES

APD

4m, 4f

103.8 (12.2)

37.5 (7.2)

14.4 (2.0)

39.9 (14.0)

APD + antidepressant

2m, 4f

105.8 (9.7)

37.6 (5.1)

15.4 (1.5)

49.4 (16.8)

I 14.29% II 28.57% III 57.14% I 22.22% II 55.56% III 11.11% IV 11.11%

comparable to the contrast detection task in encoding process, as it applies the same visual target whose contrast is modulated. It does not, however, require maintenance of visual signals over time.

Recruitment and ascertainment of the patients were similar those used in Experiment 1. The patients in the two medication groups were matched for age, sex, education and socio-economic status, and scores for BPRS ( p>0.1) (Table 2).

1.2. Experiment 2 1.2.1. Methods 1.2.1.1. Subjects. Fourteen schizophrenia patients participated in this experiment. Eight patients were taking APDs and six taking a combination of APDs and antidepressants. Medications were prescribed independently of this research study.

1.2.1.2. Stimuli. The target was similar to that used in Experiment 1 except that the direction of motion was either leftward or rightward, randomized from trial to trial (see procedure below). 1.2.1.3. Procedure. Experiment 2 implemented contrast detection as explained in Experiment 1. The procedure for direction discrimination was sim-

Fig. 2. Comparison of group thresholds in contrast detection and direction discrimination. Light bars are for the patients taking APD alone (n = 8) and dark bars for the patients taking both APDs and antidepressants (n = 6). The thresholds are represented on ordinate. The error bars indicate F 1 standard deviation.

86

S. Sheremata, Y. Chen / Schizophrenia Research 70 (2004) 81–89

ilar to that used in Experiment 1 except for following changes. First, each trial consisted of one time interval and the target was present in each interval (trial). Second, the subject’s task was to indicate the direction of the moving target (left or right) each trial as it appeared. Thus, maintenance of visual signals over time was not necessary for performing the task.

2. Results Direction discrimination thresholds did not differ significantly between the patients taking antidepressant and APD drugs (0.0018, r = 0.0005) and those taking only APDs (0.0015, r = 0.0006) (t(12,1) = 1.15, p>0.1). Contrast detection thresholds, however, were again significantly elevated in the patients taking APDs and antidepressant (0.012, r = 0.017), compared with those taking just APDs (0.0014, r = 0.003) (Wilcoxon Rank Sum(12,1) = 69, p < 0.01) (Fig. 2). The group difference in contrast detection threshold was not related to patients’ illness severity as BPRS scores were not significantly different between the patients taking antidepressant and APD medication and those taking just APD drugs (t(12,1) = 0.52, p>0.1). Neither contrast detection thresholds nor direction discrimination thresholds were correlated with BPRS scores (r = 0.07, p>0.1 and r =  0.45, p>0.05, respectively). The two patient groups were similar in the composition of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder patients.

3. Discussion The two experiments showed that combined treatment of antidepressant and APD medications made schizophrenia patients perform poorly on a contrast detection task that requires maintenance of visual signals over a short period of time. The results of these experiments are consistent with the notion that modulation of dopamine and serotonin plays an important role in memory-related visual and cognitive processing. Obviously, this study had modest sample sizes, which calls a replication of its results in a larger

group of subjects, but it nevertheless provides empirical evidence for a confounded effect of combined treatment of antidepressants and APDs at behavioral level. Moreover, in direction discrimination, a task not requiring maintenance of visual signals, the patients taking both APDs and antidepressants and those taking only APDs did not differ in performance. The result from this control task suggests that the adverse effect of combining antidepressant and APD is specific to the visual processes that involve memory. Below we discuss several possible explanations of the findings and their implications for the effect of an interaction of antidepressants and APDs on visual responses of schizophrenia patients. More broadly, the results of this study suggest that a combined use of APDs and antidepressants has unwanted consequence in memory-related behaviors. First, schizophrenia patients performed normally on direction discrimination whether they took both antidepressant and APD medications or just APDs. Because direction discrimination and contrast detection used similar visual targets and experimental procedures, the impaired contrast detection found in the patients taking both antidepressant and APD medications cannot be attributed to a generalized deficit associated with schizophrenia. The generalized deficit explanation is also inconsistent with the fact that the patients’ performance and their illness severity, as measured by BPRS, were not correlated. Second, the pharmacological effect of combining APD with antidepressant medication, as compared with using APD alone, differs in the extent to which D1 receptors are stimulated. Administration of an APD alone leads to more stimulation of D1 receptor than does co-administration of two drugs; combined use of the two psychotropic drugs attenuates the antagonism of 5HT2a, thus resulting in less D1 receptor stimulation. There may be a conceptual gap in applying contrast detection to understand the drug effect, as it is generally believed that this task is mediated in the occipital cortex if not earlier. In other words, visual processing of contrast may have been completed before it reaches the prefrontal cortex, the site at which modulation of D1 receptors occurs and working memory is mediated. However, the requirement of maintaining visual signals over time in the contrast detection task suggests an involvement of not only sensory but also working memory processes.

S. Sheremata, Y. Chen / Schizophrenia Research 70 (2004) 81–89

Third, schizophrenia patients show spatial working memory deficits (e.g. Park and Holzman, 1992). Can the contrast detection deficit, found in the patients of this study, be considered as a working memory problem? Spatial working memory operates on a time scale of several to several tens of seconds, during which information needs to be maintained on-line for further processing and recall. In the contrast detection task, visual information was maintained for less than one second. To understand the contrast detection deficit found in this study and the spatial working memory deficit reported in schizophrenic patients, we need to consider a brain mechanism that is separate from, but related to, the one underlying spatial working memory. One candidate is the iconic memory store, a short-term memory component important for maintaining basic features of visual stimuli including contrast (Magnussen, 2000). Fourth, working memory may be modulated by neurotransmission in various ways. The working memory process can be mediated in different cortical areas, depending on the nature of information and the time scale on which the information is held. Indeed, in the primary visual cortex, not only sensory but also delayed responses to visual signals have been recorded. The time scale of the delayed neuronal responses was between 500 ms and 1 s (Super et al., 2001). It appears that the range of time required for holding visual information in the contrast detection task is similar to the range of time in the delayed neuronal responses reported in visual cortex (Super et al., 2001). Therefore, the contrast detection deficit, caused by the combined use of antidepressant and APD, may be more related to the short-term memory storage of visual signals in primary visual cortex. While we do not have much knowledge about the interaction of serotonin and dopamine in the primary visual cortex, we do know that feedback from other cortical areas plays an important role in determining neuronal responses from this area (Foxe and Simpson, 2002; Super et al., 2001). Two possible mechanisms may underlie the effect of co-administration of APD and antidepressant medications on contrast detection in schizophrenia patients. First, a disruption of the APDs’ antagonism of D2 and 5HT2a by antidepressant occurs in the primary visual cortex. This explanation, however, requires a prominent presence of dopamine and serotonin receptors in this visual area. The evidence for

87

the latter receptors is not yet available. Second, disruption of the APDs’ antagonism of D2 and 5HT2a by antidepressants occurs in the cortical areas beyond the primary visual cortex. Through a feedback mechanism, the disruption in these higher areas may alter the neuronal responses in the primary visual cortex, the site where contrast detection is accomplished. This visual feedback mechanism may bridge the gap between visual processing in the occipital areas and working memory in the prefrontal areas. Foxe and Simpson (2002) indicate that feedback to the primary visual cortex from parietal and prefrontal areas can occur as quickly as 30 ms. In that study, the P1 and N1 recorded in ERP actually represent late, rather than early visual processing, unlike originally assumed. The notion that a feedback mechanism may provide a common link between working memory and visual processing appears to be a plausible interpretation for the results of this study. The co-administration of APD and antidepressant medications acts to attenuate the magnitude of regulation of D1 receptors that occurs when APDs are administered alone. This attenuation may occur in the prefrontal areas, because of the prominence of both serotonin and dopamine receptors in this cortical area. If so, the attenuation of D1 stimulation in the prefrontal areas would send a signal back to the visual areas, the location where visual working memory is based. In this study, we showed that co-administration of antidepressant and APD produced poor performance only on a visual task that requires maintenance of visual signals. Insomuch as behavioral responses and quality of life in schizophrenic patients are interrelated (Green, 1996), any strategy for drug treatment should take into account not only psychotic status but also other factors such as sensory functions. We suggest that given the fast dynamics of atypical antipsychotic drugs (Seeman and Tallerico, 1999), it may be possible to reduce the interaction of atypical antipsychotic and antidepressant drugs. Specifically, patients should be aware of the unwanted side-effect on visual perception, as shown in this paper, and consider taking antidepressants and atypical antipsychotic drugs, when needed, separately, i.e., allowing the effect of atypical antipsychotic drugs largely to dissipate before administering antidepressants. Patients may also consider selecting those antidepressants that do not act on 5HT2a receptors (e.g. Wellbutrin), if they also need to

88

S. Sheremata, Y. Chen / Schizophrenia Research 70 (2004) 81–89

take atypical antipsychotic drugs. In this regard, it is useful to study whether 5HT2a alone has an effect on memory-related visual functions, which could be done in depressed but not schizophrenic patients who take only antidepressants. Since there is evidence for the involvement of D1 receptors in the prefrontal cortex even at a delay of 1 – 1.5 s (Sawaguchi and Goldman-Rakic, 1994; Aultman and Moghaddam, 2001), the possibility of interpreting the effect of co-administration of APD and antidepressant medication based on prefrontal mechanisms cannot be excluded. Further research into the existence of such a mechanism, its time course, and its function is needed. The actions of antipsychotic drugs involve complex neurochemical and neurophysiological processes. In one study, we showed that antipsychotic drugs have little effect on motion discrimination (Chen et al., 1999). In another study, we found that schizophrenia patients taking APDs performed normally on a contrast detection task while patients taking typical antipsychotics preformed deficiently (Chen et al., 2003). Combination of different types of psychotropic drugs is an even more complicated issue. It remains to be seen whether the combined treatment of APD and antidepressant medication affects other memory-related sensory, motor, and cognitive processing. Acknowledgements We thank Drs. Philip Holzman, Deborah Levy and Steven Matthysse for the support in various stages of this study. This research was supported in part by USPHS Grants MH 61824 and 01020, by a NARSAD Young Investigator Award, by a grant from the Milton Foundation of Harvard University, and by a Rappaport Mental Health Scholar Award. References Aultman, J.M., Moghaddam, B., 2001. Distinct contributions of glutamate and dopamine receptors to temporal aspects of rodent working memory using a clinically relevant task. Psychopharmacology 153, 353 – 364. Chen, Y., Palafox, G., Nakayama, K., Levy, D.L., Matthysse, S., Holzman, P.S., 1999. Motion perception in schizophrenia. A.M.A. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 56, 149 – 154.

Chen, Y., Levy, D.L., Sheremata, S., Nakayama, K., Matthysse, S., Holzman, P.S., 2003. Effects of typical, atypical and no antipsychotic drugs on visual contrast detection in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 1 – 7. Foxe, J.J., Simpson, G.V., 2002. Flow of activation from V1 to frontal cortex in humans. A framework for defining ‘‘early’’ visual processing. Exp. Brain Res. 142 (1), 139 – 150 (Jan). Green, M.F., 1996. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am. J. Psychiatry 153, 321 – 330. Ichikawa, J., Dai, J., Meltzer, H.Y., 2001a. DOI, a 5-HT2A/2C receptor agonist, attenuates clozapine-induced cortical dopamine release. Brain Res. 907 (1 – 2), 151 – 155 (Jul.13). Ichikawa, J., Ishii, H., Bonaccorso, S., Fowler, W.L., O’Laughlin, I.A., Meltzer, H.Y., 2001b. 5-HT(2A) and D(2) receptor blockade increases cortical DA release via 5-HT(1A) receptor activation: a possible mechanism of atypical antipsychotic-induced cortical dopamine release. J. Neurochem. 76 (5), 1521 – 1531. Kuroki, T., Meltzer, H.Y., Ichikawa, J., 1999. Effects of antipsychotic drugs on extracellular dopamine levels in rat medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 288 (2), 774 – 781. Magnussen, S., 2000. Low-level memory processes in vision. Trends Neurosci. 23 (6), 247 – 251 (Jun). Moghaddam, B., Bunney, B.S., 1990. Acute effects of typical and atypical antipsychotic drugs on the release of dopamine from prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, and striatum of the rat: an in vivo microdialysis study. J. Neurochem. 54 (5), 1755 – 1760. Nomikos, G.G., Iurlo, M., Andersson, J.L., Kimura, K., Svensson, T.H., 1994. Systemic administration of amperozide, a new atypical antipsychotic drug, preferentially increases dopamine release in the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 115 (1 – 2), 147 – 156. Park, S., Holzman, P.S., 1992. Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49 (12), 975 – 982 (Dec). Pehek, E.A., Yamamoto, B.K., 1994. Differential effects of locally administered clozapine and haloperidol on dopamine efflux in the rat prefrontal cortex and caudate-putamen. J. Neurochem. 63 (6), 2118 – 2124. Poyurovsky, M., Koren, D., Gonopolsky, I., Schneidman, M., Fuchs, C., Weizman, A., Weizman, R., 2003. Effect of the 5HT2 antagonist mianserin on cognitive dysfunction in chronic schizophrenia patients: an add-on, double-blind placebo-controlled study. E. Neuropsychopharmacol. 13, 123 – 128. Rollema, H., Lu, Y., Schmidt, A.W., Sprouse, J.S., Zorn, S.H., 2000. 5-HT(1A) receptor activation contributes to ziprasidoneinduced dopamine release in the rat prefrontal cortex. Biol. Psychiatry 48 (3), 229 – 237 (1). Sawaguchi, T., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1994. The role of D1-dopamine receptor in working memory: local injections of dopamine antagonists into the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys performing an oculomotor delayed-response task. J. Neurophysiol. 71 (2), 515 – 528. Seeman, P., Tallerico, T., 1999. Rapid release of antipsychotic drugs from dopamine D2 receptors: an explanation for low receptor

S. Sheremata, Y. Chen / Schizophrenia Research 70 (2004) 81–89 occupancy and early clinical relapse upon drug withdrawal of clozapine or quetiapine. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 876 – 884. Siris, S.G., 2000. Depression in schizophrenia: perspective in the era of ‘‘Atypical’’ antipsychotic agents. Am. J. Psychiatry 157 (9), 1379 – 1389. Spitzer, R., William, J., Gibbon, M., First, M., 1994. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-Patient Edition (SCDI-P) American Psychiatric Press, Washington, D.C. Super, H., Spekreijse, H., Lamme, V.A.F., 2001. A neural correlate of working memory in the monkey primary visual cortex. Science 293 (5527), 120 – 124.

89

Volonte´, M., Monferini, E., Cerutti, M., Fodritto, F., Borsini, F., 1997. BIMG 80, a novel potential antipsychotic drug: evidence for multireceptor actions and preferential release of dopamine in prefrontal cortex. J. Neurochem. 69, 182 – 190. Westerink, B.H., Kawahara, Y., De Boer, P., Geels, C., De Vries, J.B., Wikstrom, H.V., Van Kalkeren, A., Van Vliet, B., Kruse, C.G., Long, S.K., 2001. Antipsychotic drugs classified by their effects on the release of dopamine and noradrenaline in the prefrontal cortex and striatum. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 412 (2), 127 – 138 (26).

Co-administration of atypical antipsychotics and ...

stimulation of D1 receptors than does ziprasidone, which is a more potent D2 receptor antagonist. In addition, certain antidepressants exert effects not only on serotonin, but also on dopamine and norepineph- rine. For these reasons, this study included only those subjects taking clozapine, olanzapine, or quetiapine. APD.

301KB Sizes 1 Downloads 252 Views

Recommend Documents

Atypical trajectories of number development-a neuroconstructivist ...
Atypical trajectories of number development-a neuroconstructivist perspective.pdf. Atypical trajectories of number development-a neuroconstructivist ...

Characterization of the Atypical MAPK ERK4 and Its ...
Nov 11, 2004 - ... Society for Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Inc. Printed in the U.S.A. ...... localisation in HEK293 cells, which do not express endogen-.

General Enrollment and Revalidation Checklist - Atypical HCBS.pdf ...
Page 1 of 9. General Enrollment and Revalidation Checklist – Atypical - HCBS Providers Page 1 of 9. Our mission is to improve health care access & outcomes for the people we serve while demonstrating sound. stewardship of financial resources. www.c

Atypical development of language and social communication in ...
Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome.pdf. Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu.

Atypical and typical development of visual estimation abilities.pdf ...
method: here, participants are asked to estimate. numerosities presented either in parallel or. sequentially, through the visual or auditory. modalities. Early work with rats, for instance,. indicated that when these animals were trained to. press a

Atypical development of language and social communication in ...
Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome.pdf. Atypical development of language and social ...

Atypical and typical development of visual estimation abilities.pdf ...
Page 3 of 11. Atypical and typical development of visual estimation abilities.pdf. Atypical and typical development of visual estimation abilities.pdf. Open. Extract.

Atypical Behavior of Credit: Evidence from a ...
use credit-to-GDP ratios for all three regions in question: the U.S., the euro are and Japan. To form this ratio, I use the same credit variable as in the case of the monetary VAR. Due to the normalization by GDP, which is available at lower frequenc

Typical Versus Atypical Unpacking and Superadditive ...
was unpacked into go directly to the Supreme Court and be affirmed, reversed, or ...... Koehler, D. J., White, C. M., & Grondin, R. (2003). An evidential support.

Psychological Medicine Atypical interference effect of ...
Jun 13, 2013 - Method. High-functioning adults with ASC and age- and IQ-matched healthy controls performed horizontal sinusoidal ... in social interaction, language and communication. (APA ...... Atypical network connectivity for imitation.

Movement irregularities in atypical parkinsonian ...
Nine patients with APS were compared to 9 patients with idiopathic. Parkinson's disease (PD) .... recognition software developed in our lab [7]. Qualitative ... The sensitivity of the threshold used by the program was set at 2 to match the number of

Atypical Lesion on Soft Palate.pdf
traumatic granuloma, hemangioma or blood coagulum formation after local trauma. After one week, intraoral examination. revealed absence of the lesion, which ...

Atypical basic movement kinematics in autism spectrum ...
larger test battery which, for the individuals with autism, included the biological motion ..... Supplementary material is available at Brain online. References ... mine are reflected in the kinematics of my reach-to-grasp movement. Cognition 2008a .

tips-and-tricks-of-intrascleral-fixation-of-singlepiece-and ...
complications like pigment dispersion, iris trans illumination defects, ... -of-intrascleral-fixation-of-singlepiece-and-toricintraocular-lens-2155-9570-1000514.pdf.

PROCEEDINGS OF THE COMMISSIONER AND DIRECTOR OF ...
All the Regional Joint Directors of School Education and all the District. Educational Officers in the State are informed that, it is brought to the notice of.

STUDY OF MECHANICAL AND ELECTRICAL PROPERTIES OF ...
STUDY OF MECHANICAL AND ELECTRICAL PROPERTIES OF VINYLESTER NANOCOMPOSITES.pdf. STUDY OF MECHANICAL AND ELECTRICAL ...

PROCEEDINGS OF THE COMMISSIONER AND DIRECTOR OF ...
PROCEEDINGS OF THE COMMISSIONER AND DIRECTOR OF SCHOOL. EDUCATION ANDHRA PARADESH :: HYDERABAD. Proc.Rc.No.2458/D1-4/2012. Dated: 22.04.2013. Sub: School Education – Right to Education Act -2009 –Teachers working on deputation as APO post in the

ACADEMIC POLICIES OF UNIVERSITY OF SCIENCE AND ...
ACADEMIC POLICIES OF UNIVERSITY OF SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY OF HANOI.pdf. ACADEMIC POLICIES OF UNIVERSITY OF SCIENCE AND ...

POINTWISE AND UNIFORM CONVERGENCE OF SEQUENCES OF ...
Sanjay Gupta, Assistant Professor, Post Graduate Department of .... POINTWISE AND UNIFORM CONVERGENCE OF SEQUENCES OF FUNCTIONS.pdf.

PROCEEDINGS OF THE COMMISSIONER AND DIRECTOR OF ...
All the Regional Joint Directors of School Education and all the District. Educational Officers in the State are informed that, it is brought to the notice of. Commissioner and Director of School Education, Andhra Pradesh, Hyderabad that most of the

Performance of Amplify-and-Forward and Decode-and ...
Current broadband wireless networks are characterized by large cell ... Partnership Project (3GPP) to fulfill 4G requirements specified by the International ... enhancements in radio link technology will not solve the basic problem related to ...

Comparison of Blessing of Oils and Consecration of Chrism (Latin ...
Page 1 of 6. Comparison of the Order of Blessing the Oil of Catechumens and of the Sick and of Consecrating the Chrism. 1971 Latin editio typica, 1972 ICEL, 2016 ICEL. BLESSING OF THE OIL OF THE SICK. Ordo benedicendi oleum catechumenorum et infirmor