Downloaded from rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org on 26 March 2009

Latent inhibition of predator recognition by embryonic amphibians Maud C.O Ferrari and Douglas P Chivers Biol. Lett. 2009 5, 160-162 first published online 4 December 2008 doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0641

References

This article cites 17 articles, 2 of which can be accessed free

Subject collections

Articles on similar topics can be found in the following collections

http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/5/2/160.full.html#ref-list-1

behaviour (387 articles) developmental biology (49 articles) ecology (465 articles)

Email alerting service

Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the top right-hand corner of the article or click here

To subscribe to Biol. Lett. go to: http://rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org/subscriptions

This journal is © 2009 The Royal Society

Downloaded from rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org on 26 March 2009

Much of the research on embryonic learning has tested whether pre-exposure to a chemical stimulus subsequently results in a preference of the individual for that stimulus as adult (Sneddon et al. 1998). Recently, Mathis et al. (2008) have been the first to show embryonic learning of predator recognition, reporting that amphibian embryos acquire recognition of future predators through simultaneous pairing of injured tadpole cues with a novel predator odour. Natural selection should favour mechanisms and individuals able to recognize predators upon their first encounter. While this is the first indication that learned predator recognition is possible at such an early age, nothing is known about the ability of embryos to similarly learn to recognize species as nonpredators. Failing to recognize and respond to an animal as a predator is very different from recognizing an animal as a non-predator. Recognizing the animal as a non-predator means that the prey may be prevented (in the short term) from subsequently learning that the animal is a threat. In this study, we exposed embryonic wood frogs (Rana sylvatica) to the odour of tiger salamanders (Ambystoma tigrinum) in the absence of any risk reinforcement. We then attempted to teach the wood frogs, as larvae, to recognize salamander odour as threatening through the simultaneous exposure of salamander odour and conspecific alarm cues. If frog embryos learned that the salamander was non-threatening (i.e. through latent inhibition, see Barad et al. 2004), we predicted that the larvae would fail to subsequently learn to recognize the salamander as a threat. If embryos detected salamander odour but did not learn it as a non-predator, then larval wood frogs should associate salamander odour with predation risk through conditioning with chemical cues from injured conspecifics.

Biol. Lett. (2009) 5, 160–162 doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0641 Published online 4 December 2008

Animal behaviour

Latent inhibition of predator recognition by embryonic amphibians Maud C. O. Ferrari* and Douglas P. Chivers Department of Biology, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada S7N 5E2 *Author for correspondence ([email protected]).

To avoid being captured, prey animals need to be able to distinguish predators from nonpredators. Recent studies have shown that amphibians can learn to recognize their future predators while in the egg. Here, we investigated whether amphibians would similarly be able to learn to recognize non-predators while in the egg. We exposed newly laid wood frog eggs to the odour of tiger salamander or a water control daily for 5 days. After hatching, the wood frog larvae were raised for two weeks at which time we tried to condition them to recognize the salamander as a predator. Larvae were exposed to injured conspecific cues paired with salamander odour, a well-established mode of learning for aquatic prey. When subsequently tested for their response to salamander odour, the larvae pre-exposed to water as embryos showed significant anti-predator responses. However, larvae pre-exposed to the salamander odour as embryos showed no learning of the predator, indicating that they had already learned to recognize the salamander as a non-predator. These results indicate that amphibian embryos can (i) learn to recognize stimuli as non-threatening and (ii) remember it for at least two weeks. The widespread ability of prey to learn to recognize non-predators might explain the persistence of injured conspecific cues as a reliable mechanism for learned predator recognition.

2. MATERIAL AND METHODS (a) Test subjects and stimulus preparation All eggs and tadpoles used in this experiment were collected from a single pond in Central Alberta, Canada in May 2008. Male and female wood frogs breed only once a season, hence eggs obtained from different clutches are probably unrelated ( Halverson et al. 2006). Our previous work has shown that wood frogs from this pond do not have ‘innate’ recognition of their predators (e.g. Ferrari & Chivers 2008). The salamander odour used for the egg treatment phase was obtained by putting one healthy adult tiger salamander (14.1 cm snout–vent length) into 500 ml of conditioned well water for 24 hours. The water was changed every day.

Keywords: predator recognition; latent inhibition; embryo; wood frog Rana sylvatica

1. INTRODUCTION Owing to the unforgiving nature of predation, prey have evolved sophisticated behavioural responses to predators (Lima & Dill 1990). A prerequisite for such responses is for prey to distinguish predators from non-predators. While some prey species have an innate ability to recognize predators (Goth 2001; Berejikian et al. 2003), some require learning to recognize their potential predators as threatening. Several learning modes allow the acquisition of predator recognition. In aquatic organisms, amphibians (Woody & Mathis 1998; Mirza et al. 2006), fishes (Chivers & Smith 1994; Wisenden et al. 2008) and a number of invertebrates (Hazlett 2003; Ferrari et al. 2008) acquire predator recognition through simultaneous pairing of chemical cues from injured conspecifics with the sight, odour or sound of a novel predator. Received 4 November 2008 Accepted 18 November 2008

(b) Experimental set-up Five freshly laid egg clutches were collected and divided into subclutches of approximately 50–60 eggs each. Two sub-clutches from each of the five clutches were then transferred into two 3.5 l buckets filled with 3 l of well water. The sub-clutches consisted of a single mass of eggs with the egg jelly intact. The eggs were at Gosner developmental stage 10–11 (Gosner 1960). At this stage, the neural tube is not yet formed. (c) Experimental procedure (i) Embryonic pre-exposure phase The two sub-clutches from each clutch were randomly assigned to one of the two treatments: 1, water: 20 ml of well water and 2, salamander odour: 20 ml of salamander odour. Hence, we treated 10 buckets, five receiving the water treatment and five receiving the salamander odour treatment. Eggs were treated daily at 15.00 for 5 days; the stimuli were slowly injected on the side of the buckets to minimize disturbance to the eggs. At 17.00, a 100 per cent water change was performed. Eggs were treated until the embryos appeared fully formed (approx. Gosner stage 22–24) but had not hatched. The treatments stopped after day 5. Embryos started hatching the following morning and all the

160

This journal is q 2008 The Royal Society

Downloaded from rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org on 26 March 2009

Latent inhibition of predator recognition

M. C. O. Ferrari & D. P. Chivers

161

mean (± s.e.) proportion change in line crosses

0.2 0 – 0.2 – 0.4 – 0.6 – 0.8 W + (W + SO)

W + ( IC + SO)

SO + ( W + SO)

SO + ( IC + SO)

Figure 1. Mean (Gs.e.) proportion change in line crosses for tadpoles exposed to salamander odour. Wood frogs were preexposed for 5 days to either water (W) or salamander odour (SO) during their embryonic development and later conditioned as tadpoles to learn to recognize salamander as a threat through the pairing of injured conspecific cues and salamander odour (ICCSO) or through a control of water paired with salamander odour (WCSO). embryos were hatched by the evening. Tadpoles were provided with rabbit food and the water was partially changed every second day. Tadpoles were raised for 12 days prior to being tested. (ii) Larval conditioning phase Six tadpoles (Gosner stage 25) from each bucket were arbitrarily chosen and placed individually in 0.5 l plastic cups filled with well water. Three of the tadpoles were subsequently exposed to 5 ml of injured conspecific cues paired with 10 ml of salamander odour (true conditioning), while the other three were exposed to 5 ml of water paired with 10 ml of salamander odour (control). The injured conspecific cues were obtained by crushing one tadpole in 5 ml of water using a mortar and pestle. The salamander odour was obtained by soaking one salamander in 2 l of water for 24 hours. Using a single salamander could have limitations if there is considerable variation in the odour signature of individual salamanders. While variation in the signatures may lead to small variation in the intensity of responses of the tadpoles, it probably will not result in dramatic changes in whether the salamander is recognized or not. Two hours after the conditioning phase, a 100 per cent water change was conducted on all the cups, and food was provided for the tadpoles. (iii) Larval testing phase After 24 hours, all the tadpoles were tested for their responses to salamander odour. The salamander odour was obtained using the same methodology as the day before. Tadpoles were tested using a well-established protocol (e.g. Ferrari & Chivers 2008). Accordingly, the trials consisted of a 4 min pre-stimulus followed by a 4 min post-stimulus injection period, during which the activity of the tadpoles was recorded. The two periods were separated by a 30 s injection period, during which 10 ml of salamander odour was slowly introduced on the side of the cup. Tadpoles decrease activity in response to predation cues, hence a line was drawn in the middle of the cup and the number of line crosses was counted during the two observation periods. The trials were performed outdoors (nZ12–15 treatmentK1), between 14.30 and 18.30. The order of the treatments was randomized throughout the day. The observer was blind to the treatments. (d) Statistical analysis We calculated the change in the proportion of line crosses from the pre-stimulus baseline. The data were normally distributed but heteroscedastic. Hence, we analysed the data using a non-parametric ANOVA approach (Scheirer-Ray-Hare extension of the Kruskal– Wallis test (Sokal & Rohlf 2003)). The effects of ‘embryonic preexposure’ (water versus salamander odour) and ‘conditioning’ (with water or injured conspecific cues) were treated as fixed factors, whereas ‘clutch’ was included as a random factor, to account for the possibility of maternal effects. Owing to a significant interaction between the two factors, subsequent non-parametric ANOVAs were performed to further investigate the nature of the interaction.

3. RESULTS The three-way ANOVA revealed an interaction between embryonic pre-exposure and conditioning factors (F1,4.4Z72.6, p!0.002; figure 1). When looking at Biol. Lett. (2009)

the effect of ‘conditioning’ on each ‘pre-exposure’ level, the responses of tadpoles pre-exposed to water as embryos were affected by the conditioning treatment received as tadpoles (conditioning: F1,4Z23.3, pZ0.008; clutch: F4,4Z0.9, pO0.5; interaction: F4,19Z0.8, pO0.5). Specifically, tadpoles receiving salamander odour paired with injured cues responded to the salamander odour with stronger anti-predator behaviour than tadpoles receiving the salamander odour paired with water. However, the responses of tadpoles pre-exposed to salamander odour as embryos were not affected by the conditioning treatment received as tadpoles (conditioning: F1,4Z0.9, pO0.3; clutch: F4,4Z1.4, pO0.3; interaction: F4,16Z1.0, pO0.4). When looking at the effect of embryonic pre-exposure on each conditioning level, the treatment received during the embryonic pre-exposure did not affect the responses to salamander odour of tadpoles conditioned with water paired with salamander odour (pre-exposure: F1,4Z0.5, pO0.5; clutch: F4,4Z4.8, pO0.05; interaction: F4,17Z0.6, pO0.6). However, when tadpoles were conditioned with injured cues paired with salamander odour, embryonic pre-exposure had an effect on their responses to salamander (preexposure: F1,4Z21.7, pZ0.009; clutch: F4,4Z0.5, pO0.7; interaction: F4,18Z0.7, pO0.6). Specifically, tadpoles pre-exposed to water as embryos responded with a stronger intensity to salamander odour than tadpoles pre-exposed to salamander odour as embryos.

4. DISCUSSION Our results clearly demonstrate that wood frog embryos pre-exposed to salamander odour without risk reinforcement learned that the salamander was a non-predator, as indicated by their failure to subsequently learn to recognize the salamander as threatening through the pairing of salamander odour and injured conspecific cues. This ‘latent inhibition’ phenomenon has been demonstrated in adult crayfish (Acquistapace et al. 2003) and adult fathead minnows (Ferrari & Chivers 2006). This is the first time, to our knowledge, that this phenomenon has been demonstrated in embryos. In addition, previous demonstrations of latent inhibition consisted of the

Downloaded from rsbl.royalsocietypublishing.org on 26 March 2009

162

M. C. O. Ferrari & D. P. Chivers

Latent inhibition of predator recognition

pre-exposure period, immediately followed by the conditioning and testing periods. Here, wood frogs experienced a 15-day lag between the end of the preexposure and the conditioning phase. Understanding how much information is needed to override nonpredator recognition is a fascinating area for future research. To optimize their fitness, prey trade-off predator avoidance against other activities. While underresponding to predation threats may have immediate and irreversible consequences (i.e. death), overresponding to non-predators can be costly as a result of missed foraging or mating opportunities. Hence, it is critical for the prey to respond to the appropriate threats and not to respond to non-risky stimuli. The ability of prey to learn to recognize non-predators may explain the success of the secondary evolution of ‘alarm cues’ as public information. Owing to the high efficiency of learning through pairing with injured conspecific cues, it seems likely that prey could frequently learn to recognize some non-threatening stimuli as dangerous through chance alone. Thus, mechanisms such as latent inhibition may maintain the proportion of erroneous learning events at a low level, explaining the persistence of conditioning with alarm cues as a reliable mechanism for learned predator recognition. All work reported was in accordance with the Animal Care Committee Protocol no. 20060014 from University of Saskatchewan. Research funding was provided to D. Chivers through the NSERC.

Acquistapace, P., Hazlett, B. A. & Gherardi, F. 2003 Unsuccessful predation and learning of predator cues by crayfish. J. Crust. Biol. 23, 364–370. (doi:10.1651/02780372(2003)023[0364:UPALOP]2.0.CO;2) Barad, M., Blouin, A. M. & Cain, C. K. 2004 Like extinction, latent inhibition of conditioned fear in mice is blocked by systemic inhibition of L-type voltage-gated calcium channels. Learn. Mem. 11, 536–539. (doi:10. 1101/lm.78304) Berejikian, B. A., Tezak, E. P. & LaRae, A. L. 2003 Innate and enhanced predator recognition in hatchery-reared chinook salmon. Environ. Biol. Fishes 67, 241–251. (doi:10.1023/A:1025887015436) Chivers, D. P. & Smith, R. J. F. 1994 Fathead minnows, Pimephales promelas, acquire predator recognition when alarm substance is associated with the sight of unfamiliar fish. Anim. Behav. 48, 597–605. (doi:10.1006/anbe. 1994.1279)

Biol. Lett. (2009)

Ferrari, M. C. O. & Chivers, D. P. 2006 The role of latent inhibition in acquired predator recognition by fathead minnows. Can. J. Zool. 84, 505–509. (doi:10. 1139/Z06-027) Ferrari, M. C. O. & Chivers, D. P. 2008 Cultural learning of predator recognition in mixed-species assemblages of frogs: the effect of tutor-to-observer ratio. Anim. Behav. 75, 1921–1925. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.10.037) Ferrari, M. C. O., Messier, F. & Chivers, D. P. 2008 Threat-sensitive learning of predators by larval mosquitoes Culex restuans. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62, 1079–1083. (doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0535-7) Gosner, K. L. 1960 A simplified table for staging anuran embryos and larvae with notes on identification. Herpetologica 16, 183–190. Goth, A. 2001 Innate predator-recognition in Australian brush-turkey (Alectura lathami, Megapodiidae) hatchlings. Behaviour 138, 117–136. (doi:10.1163/156853901 750077826) Halverson, M. A., Skelly, D. K. & Caccone, A. 2006 Inbreeding linked to amphibian survival in the wild but not in the laboratory. J. Hered. 97, 499–507. (doi:10. 1093/jhered/esl019) Hazlett, B. A. 2003 Predator recognition and learned irrelevance in the crayfish Orconectes virilis. Ethology 109, 765–780. (doi:10.1046/j.1439-0310.2003.00916.x) Lima, S. L. & Dill, L. M. 1990 Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can. J. Zool. 68, 619–640. (doi:10.1139/z90-092) Mathis, A., Ferrari, M. C. O., Windel, N., Messier, F. & Chivers, D. P. 2008 Learning by embryos and the ghost of predation future. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 2603–2607. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0754) Mirza, R. S., Ferrari, M. C. O., Kiesecker, J. M. & Chivers, D. P. 2006 Responses of American toad tadpoles to predation cues: behavioural response thresholds, threatsensitivity and acquired predation recognition. Behaviour 143, 877–889. (doi:10.1163/156853906778017926) Sneddon, H., Hadden, R. & Hepper, P. G. 1998 Chemosensory learning in the chicken embryo. Physiol. Behav. 64, 133–139. (doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00037-7) Sokal, R. R. & Rohlf, F. J. 2003 Biometry: the principles and practice of statistics in biological research, 3rd edn. New York, NY: Freeman and Co. Wisenden, B. D., Pogatshnik, J., Gibson, D., Bonacci, L., Schumacher, A. & Willett, A. 2008 Sound the alarm: learned association of predation risk with novel auditory stimuli by fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) and glowlight tetras (Hemigrammus erythrozonus) after single simultaneous pairings with conspecific chemical alarm cues. Environ. Biol. Fishes 81, 141–147. (doi:10.1007/ s10641-006-9181-6) Woody, D. R. & Mathis, A. 1998 Acquired recognition of chemical stimuli from an unfamiliar predator: associative learning by adult newts, Notophthalmus viridescens. Copeia 4, 1027–1031. (doi:10.2307/1447352)

amphibians Latent inhibition of predator recognition by ...

Dec 4, 2008 - Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in .... in response to predation cues, hence a line was drawn in the middle.

117KB Sizes 1 Downloads 275 Views

Recommend Documents

The role of latent inhibition in acquired predator ...
tween the two cues. For example, Chivers et al. (1996) dem- .... to the non-normality of the line cross data, we conducted nonparametric Mann–Whitney U tests to ...

Friend or foe? The role of latent inhibition in predator ... - Springer Link
Apr 26, 2011 - based on a single conditioning event whereby they are exposed to the unknown predator at the same time as alarm cues released from injured ...

Differential retention of predator recognition by juvenile ...
Here, we investigated if the memory window associated with learned recognition of predators by juvenile rainbow trout was fixed or variable. Specifically, we.

Differential retention of predator recognition by juvenile ...
(1 Department of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California, Davis, CA,. 95616 USA; 2 Department of Biology, ... Also available online - www.brill.nl/beh ..... species to identifying 'classes' of predators. In other words, their ...

Generalization of learned predator recognition: an ...
May 22, 2007 - 5 ml of either alarm cues or dechlorinated tap water and. 20 ml of lake trout ... of fishes on either change in shoaling index (c4. 2. Z3.7,. pZ0.44) ...

Threat-sensitive generalization of predator recognition ...
May 27, 2009 - These results support previous results by Mirza et al. (2006), whose .... Predator Recognition Continuum Hypothesis proposed by. Ferrari et al.

Cultural learning of predator recognition in mixed ...
final acceptance 26 October 2007; published online 15 April 2008; MS. number: A10807). Animals that live in social ... However, the degree to which social learning influ- ences behaviour in other taxa ..... Griffin, A. S. & Evans, C. S. 2003.

A Latent Semantic Pattern Recognition Strategy for an ...
Abstract—Target definition is a process aimed at partitioning the potential ...... blog texts and its application to event discovery,” Data Mining and Knowledge ...

Inhibition by Peptides of Amino Acid Uptake by ... - Europe PMC
Department ofMicrobiology, University ofGeorgia, Athens, Georgia 30602. Received 12 September 1983/Accepted 29 December 1983. To investigate the regulatory interactions of amino acid transport and incorporation, we determined the effects of dipeptide

Mechanism of CYP2C9 Inhibition by Flavones and ... - Semantic Scholar
Dec 11, 2008 - ABSTRACT: This article describes an in vitro investigation of the inhibition of cytochrome P450 (P450) 2C9 by a series of flavonoids made up of flavones (flavone, 6-hydroxyflavone, 7-hydroxyflavone, chrysin, ba- icalein, apigenin, lute

Inhibition of masked primes as revealed by saccade ...
Jan 17, 2012 - The most common effect a prime has on the response to the target is that .... Illustration of the hypothesized activity in the saccade motor map (along ... because of the large number of trials (>50%) that had to be removed on.

Structural basis for inhibition of human PNP by ...
179 cess code: 1ULB) indicates a large movement in the. 180 Lys244 side .... 248 [3] S. Banthia, J.A. Montgomery, H.G. Johnson, G.M. Walsh, In. 249 vivo and in ...

Inhibition of hippocampal synaptic transmission by ...
Nov 7, 2005 - 10 ms. 2000 pA. 60. 100. Fig. 1 Inhibition of hippocampal synaptic transmission by .... A role of Ral in synaptic vesicle traffic was first suggested.

Inhibition of masked primes as revealed by saccade ...
Jan 17, 2012 - Interest- ingly, we found a dissociation between the directions of masked priming and .... return and saccade curvature by comparing the time-course at which they ...... Learning under anesthesia: checking the light in the fridge ...

Inhibition of constitutive NF-jB activity by IjBaM ...
Sciences, The University of Texas, Houston, TX 77030, USA; 3Department of Gastrointestinal Medical ..... studies suggest that VEGF is the best-validated target.

pdf-1423\predator-cities-3-infernal-devices-predator-citites-by-philip ...
pdf-1423\predator-cities-3-infernal-devices-predator-citites-by-philip-reeve.pdf. pdf-1423\predator-cities-3-infernal-devices-predator-citites-by-philip-reeve.pdf.

Inhibition of Netrin-Mediated Axon Attraction by a ...
organization of brain energy metabolism dur- ing focal neural .... ance receptors in AVM (green) guide axons toward ventral UNC-6/netrin. (blue) and away from ...

Inhibition of the 26 S Proteasome by Polyubiquitin ...
noted, other reagents were purchased from Sigma. Protein iodination with chloramine T was carried ... the NH2-terminal sequence is MHHHHHHGEFQ, where Q corresponds to Q2 in wild type human ubiquitin. .... h, were determined by examining reaction aliq

Predator Panic.pdf
“To Catch a Predator,” Dateline reporter Chris Hansen claimed that “the. scope of the problem is ... Main menu. Displaying Predator Panic.pdf. Page 1 of 9.

Mechanism of Ubiquitin Recognition by the CUE Domain of Vps9p
May 29, 2003 - CUE domain is a dimeric domain-swapped variant of .... 100%. 99.5 a Statistics are shown for the peak wavelength of the Selenomethionine MAD datasets. Statistics for ..... from the CUE dimer and free ubiquitin are identical to.

Mechanism of Ubiquitin Recognition by the CUE Domain of Vps9p
May 29, 2003 - Only the ubiquitin making functionally rele- vant contacts with the ..... (F) Schematic illustration of the lattice relationships. (G) of the second .... Department of Energy, Basic Energy Sciences, Office of Science, novel ubiquitin .