PS YC HOLOGICA L SC IENCE

Research Article

Differentiating Social and Personal Power Opposite Effects on Stereotyping, but Parallel Effects on Behavioral Approach Tendencies Joris Lammers,1 Janka I. Stoker,2 and Diederik A. Stapel1 1

Tilburg University and 2University of Groningen

ABSTRACT—How does power affect behavior? We posit that

(BWUS PSCI 2479 Webpdf:=10/22/2009 03:43:42 200528 Bytes 7 PAGES n operator=S.arsathali) 10/22/2009 3:43:48 PM

this depends on the type of power. We distinguish between social power (power over other people) and personal power (freedom from other people) and argue that these two types of power have opposite associations with independence and interdependence. We propose that when the distinction between independence and interdependence is relevant, social power and personal power will have opposite effects; however, they will have parallel effects when the distinction is irrelevant. In two studies (an experimental study and a large field study), we demonstrate this by showing that social power and personal power have opposite effects on stereotyping, but parallel effects on behavioral approach.

PSCI

How does power affect behavior? This is an important question in psychology, given that we live in a world with extremely salient power differentials. Presidents can decide on the economic fate of millions, CEOs can hire or fire thousands of employees, and generals can make decisions that determine whether thousands of soldiers live or die. If people’s behavior is affected by their power, then it is crucial to know how and why. In the current research, we argue that to answer how power affects behavior, it is necessary to take a closer look at the concept of power itself. We propose that power can refer to different things, which in turn have different effects. In the relevant literature, however, power is traditionally treated as a single construct, and the difference between various aspects of power is ignored. The multitude of competing and diverging power definitions already Address correspondence to Joris Lammers, Tilburg Institute for Behavioral Economics Research (TIBER), Department of Social Psychology, Tilburg University, Warandelaan 2, 5037 AB Tilburg, The Netherlands, e-mail: [email protected].

Volume ]]]—Number ]]

2479

indicates that a monolithic power concept is too restrictive (Fiske & Berdahl, 2007). A close look at these many definitions of power suggests that there are two main types. SOCIAL POWER AND PERSONAL POWER

One group of definitions describes power as the ability of a person to influence others and make them do things they would not do otherwise (e.g., Weber, 1914/1978). In this case, power means exercising control over other people. This type of power is often called social power (Overbeck, in press; Van Dijke & Poppe, 2006). An example of social power is the power of managers over their employees. A second group of definitions describes power as the ability to do and get what you want, without being influenced by others (e.g. Cartwright, 1959; Emerson, 1962; French & Raven, 1959). In this case, power is the ability to ignore the influence of others, to control one’s own outcomes, and to be personally independent. This type of power is often called personal power (Galinsky, Magee, Gruenfeld, Whitson, & Liljenquist, 2008; Overbeck, in press; Van Dijke & Poppe, 2006). Examples of personal power are the types of power that money or knowledge brings. Someone who has a lot of money or knowledge is less dependent on others than someone who lacks these things. In everyday life, certain positions bring more social power, and others more personal power. For example, successful artists have mainly personal power, as they can decide when and what they paint, whereas army officers have a lot of social power over their troops, who must closely follow their orders. Many positions of power entail both social and personal power. Yet this should not be taken to mean that social and personal power cannot be distinguished as separate concepts, with separate effects. In this article, we aim to show that one can make the distinction between social and personal power. In addition,

1

Copyright r 2009 Association for Psychological Science

P S C I

2 4 7 9

Journal Name

Manuscript No.

B

Dispatch: 22.10.09

Journal: PSCI

Author Received:

No. of pages: 7

CE: Mahendra PE: TMS/Jay

Differentiating Social and Personal Power

(BWUS PSCI 2479 Webpdf:=10/22/2009 03:43:42 200528 Bytes 7 PAGES n operator=S.arsathali) 10/22/2009 3:43:48 PM

we aim to show that sometimes one should make such a distinction, as the two types of power can have different and even opposite effects. A number of researchers have already used the social/personal power distinction. However, all did so to help focus attention on an aspect of power that they felt had been neglected. For example, Van Dijke and Poppe (2006) showed that in general, people prefer to increase their personal power (i.e., independence from others) but have no special desire for social power (over others). Galinsky et al. (2008) argued that power decreases people’s dependence on norms, which is an effect of personal rather than social power. Similarly, Galinsky, Gruenfeld, and Magee (2003) speculated that power has two elements—responsibility and self-interest—that perhaps have different effects. The authors mentioned in this paragraph have limited themselves, however, to speculating about whether effects in their research were due more to social or to personal power. They did not directly compare social power and personal power as independent constructs. We claim that it is important to make such direct comparisons because social power and personal power are conceptualized in completely different ways. Personal power is power over oneself and freedom from the influence of others. People who experience a substantial amount of personal power are unconstrained by and independent from others (Cartwright, 1959; Emerson, 1962). As a result, people high in personal power do not need to bother or care about other people in their social environment. Social power, on the other hand, is associated with interdependence rather than with independence (Arendt, 1969; Parsons, 1967). It is therefore strongly linked with the need for responsibility. We propose that the effects of social and personal power can differ if the distinction between independence and interdependence is relevant for the variable of interest.

PSCI

STEREOTYPING: OPPOSITE EFFECTS OF SOCIAL AND PERSONAL POWER

Several authors have already observed that power can have different effects on stereotyping: Power sometimes increases stereotyping and sometimes decreases it (Chen, Ybarra, & Kiefer, 2004; Overbeck & Park, 2001, 2006). It is, however, unclear why this is the case. Overbeck and Park (2006) suggested that some power relations are more people centered than others, but it is still unclear what exactly distinguishes more people-centered power from less people-centered power. We posit that the distinction between social and personal power is essential here and can improve our understanding of the relation between power and stereotyping. Because personal power means freedom from others, we argue that it increases stereotyping. People with a lot of personal power will therefore be less inclined to spend the extra effort to individuate and make sense of others. Instead, they will simply rely on automatic cognition, in particular, on stereotypes (Fiske,

2

2479

1993; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Neuberg & Fiske, 1987). Yet social power means responsibility over other people. Responsible exercise of power requires care and consideration in dealing with others. For example, even though managers have power over their employees, this usually does not mean that they stereotype them. Instead, they are motivated to pay extra attention to understand what their employees are like and what they think (Bass, 1998). This is why social power should decrease stereotyping (Fiske & Neuberg, 1990; Neuberg & Fiske, 1987). This analysis of the link between personal and social power and stereotyping effects fits nicely with how previous studies empirically approached the effect of power on stereotyping. Studies that found a positive effect of power on stereotyping typically manipulated power in independent settings—associated with personal power. In these studies, participants in a high-power condition are typically given the opportunity to select anonymous alleged job applicants, who they do not meet or know and with whom they have no sense of interdependence. They are also completely free and independent in how they select candidates. Any sense of responsibility is therefore blocked. See, for example, studies that use the Springfield evaluation paradigm (Fiske & De´pret, 1996; Goodwin, Gubin, Fiske, & Yzerbyt, 2000) and studies that use the work group paradigm (Fiske & De´pret, 1996). In contrast, studies that found a negative effect of power on stereotyping typically manipulated power in interdependent settings—associated with social power. In these experiments, powerful people operate as agents of an organization, responsible for their subordinates’ performance as members of an interdependent collective. For example, they adopt the role of a professor versus a student (Overbeck & Park, 2001, Study 1) or of a judge in a legal case (Overbeck & Park, 2001, Study 2), two roles that have strong associations with responsibility. Another study that reported decreased stereotyping among the powerful (Chen et al., 2004) obtained these effects by priming the experience of power but predominantly used social-power stimuli (i.e., authority, executive, influence, control, and boss) and only one personal-power stimulus (money). APPROACH: PARALLEL EFFECTS OF SOCIAL AND PERSONAL POWER

Importantly, we propose that effects of power are only contingent on the distinction between social and personal power, if the distinction between independence and interdependence is relevant for that effect. One area where the distinction between independence and interdependence is irrelevant to the effect of power is behavioral approach (Anderson & Berdahl, 2002; Galinsky et al., 2003; Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003; Smith & Bargh, 2008). As Keltner et al. have explained, power increases behavioral approach tendencies both because high power is associated with the freedom to act without interference

Volume ]]]—Number ]]

Joris Lammers, Janka I. Stoker, and Diederik A. Stapel

(i.e., personal power) and because high power is associated with control over other people’s rewards (i.e., social power). Social and personal power both contribute to the effect. STUDY 1: AN EXPERIMENT

To test our predictions, we performed two studies. In Study 1, we primed social power and personal power and compared their effects on stereotyping and behavioral approach. Method

(BWUS PSCI 2479 Webpdf:=10/22/2009 03:43:42 200528 Bytes 7 PAGES n operator=S.arsathali) 10/22/2009 3:43:48 PM

Participants and Design Participants comprised 113 university students (73women and 40 men with a mean age of 21.4 years) who took part in exchange for h7. Participants were randomly assigned to one of six conditions in a 3 (manipulation: social power, personal power, or control)  2 (dependent variable: stereotyping or approach) between-participants design.

PSCI

Procedure Participants first completed an adapted version of the experiential power prime (Galinsky et al., 2003). In the social-power condition, participants were asked to recall an experience ’’in which you had power over others, where you controlled and directed other people. This means that you could determine what these others should do or what they would get.’’ We asked participants in the personal-power condition to recall an experience ’’in which you personally had power, where you were independent from the influence of others. This means that you could fully determine what you yourself would do or get.’’ Participants in the control condition recalled the last time they went shopping. All the participants used a sheet of paper with 20 lines to write about this experience. There was no difference among conditions on the amount recalled (overall M 5 9.5 lines completed), F(2, 109) < 0.5, p > .6. Measures Behavioral Approach. Half of the participants were randomly selected to complete a 12-item behavioral approach scale (e.g., ’’Currently, I would like to do my best to get the things I want’’). Participants rated each item on a 9-point scale ranging from fully disagree, 1, to fully agree, 9 (a 5 .71). This scale reliably measures actual approach-related behavior tendencies (Lammers, Galinsky, Gordijn, & Otten, 2008). Stereotyping. The other half of the participants completed a Donald paradigm (see Higgins, Rholes, & Jones, 1977), to measure the degree to which they were inclined to stereotype. Participants read a short story (18 lines of text) about a girl named Petra, who behaved in an ambiguously stereotypically female manner (e.g., behaving in a way that may be interpreted as showing dependency on her boyfriend, as being indecisive, and as exhibiting strong empathy). Next, they rated Petra on 10

Volume ]]]—Number ]]

2479

traits, all stereotypical for women (i.e., dependent, social, sensitive, naive, dedicated, caring, modest, kind, covetous, and friendly), on a 9-point scale ranging from not at all, 1, and very much, 9 (see Banaji, Hardin, & Rothman, 1993; Stapel & Koomen, 2001). Manipulation Checks. Finally, all participants completed three types of manipulation checks, all administered on 9-point scales, ranging from not at all, 1, to very much, 9. Four items measured feelings of personal power (e.g., ‘‘I felt independent’’; a 5 .84), four items measured feelings of social power (e.g., ‘‘I felt in charge of others’’; a 5 .83), and four items measured unspecific, general power (e.g., ‘‘I felt powerful’’; a 5 .96).

Results Manipulation Checks Factor analysis on the 12 manipulation-check items yielded three clearly identifiable constructs (general, social, and personal power), each with strong factor loadings on their respective components and low loadings on other components. Results of the factor analysis are shown in Table S1 in the Supporting Information available on-line (see p. XXX).1 General power (averaged over four items) correlated strongly with social power (r 5 .68, p < .001) and correlated weakly with personal power (r 5 .27, p 5 .06). As expected, social power and personal power were uncorrelated (r 5 .12, p 5 .43). Preliminary analyses also showed that social power and personal power were selectively manipulated and general power was jointly manipulated. That is, personal-power scores were highest in the personal-power condition, and social-power scores were highest in the social-power condition; in addition, general power scores were higher in the personal- and socialpower conditions than in the control conditions. Details of these analyses are displayed in Table S2 in the Supporting Information available on-line. Stereotyping An analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the effect of the experimental manipulations (social, personal, and control) on participants’ stereotyping showed a robust effect, F(2, 58) 5 8.68, p < .001, Zp 2 ¼ :23. Contrast analyses showed that—in keeping with predictions—personal power increased stereotyping (M 5 6.07, SD 5 0.47), compared with the control condition (M 5 5.74, SD 5 0.47), t(58) 5 2.21, p 5 .03, whereas—also in keeping with predictions—social power decreased participants’ tendency to stereotype (M 5 5.44, SD 5 0.49), compared with the control condition, t(58) 5 2.01, p 5 .049. The difference between social power and personal power was highly significant, t(58) 5 4.17, p < .001. 1

Within-condition factor analyses showed similar patterns.

3

Differentiating Social and Personal Power

Approach An ANOVA on the effect of the experimental manipulation of power on participants’ behavioral approach tendencies showed a robust effect, F(2, 49) 5 3.71, p 5 .03, Zp 2 ¼ :13. In keeping with predictions, contrast analyses showed that personal power increased participants’ approach tendencies (M 5 5.68, SD 5 0.74), compared with the control condition (M 5 5.11, SD 5 0.87), t(49) 5 2.56, p 5 .01, and social power likewise increased approach tendencies (M 5 5.79, SD 5 0.74) compared with the control condition, t(49) 5 2.11, p 5 .04. As expected, social power and personal power did not differ, t(49) 5 0.41, p 5 .68.

STUDY 2: A CROSS-SECTIONAL FIELD STUDY

Although this experimental manipulation in Study 1 had many advantages, it had one drawback: An important element in our predictions was that social and personal power (as different aspects of power) can have opposite effects, even though they are in reality often positively correlated. To show this, we decided to measure social power and personal power in existing relations and study how they are related to stereotyping and approach.

(BWUS PSCI 2479 Webpdf:=10/22/2009 03:43:42 200528 Bytes 7 PAGES n operator=S.arsathali) 10/22/2009 3:43:48 PM

Method

PSCI

Discussion As predicted, we found that social and personal power had parallel effects on approach but opposite effects on stereotyping. That is, personal power increases stereotyping, social power decreases stereotyping, and both personal and social power increase approach. Furthermore, our manipulation checks showed that both social power and personal power are independent constructs that do not strongly correlate with each other but do correlate positively with general power. These manipulation checks also showed that social and personal power can be reliably primed as independent constructs; our personal-power manipulation selectively increased personal power, our socialpower manipulation selectively increased social power, and both manipulations increased general power.2 To rule out that that these effects were caused by the fact that our power instruction might have primed a self- versus otherorientation, we conducted an additional study, in which 103 university students (73 women and 30 men, with a mean age of 19.7 years) participated in exchange for h7. The design was similar to that of Study 1, but instead of the dependent variables, we administered (in random order) five other- versus self-orientation scales: the Empathy subscale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis, 1983), the Inclusion of the Other in the Self Scale (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992), the Concern for Others subscale of the student version of the Comparative Emphasis Scale (Ravlin & Meglino, 1987), the Relational-Interdependent Self-Construal Scale (Cross, Bacon, & Morris, 2000), and the Individualistic and Altruistic subscales of the Social Value Orientation Scale (Van Lange, 1999). ANOVAs revealed no effect of our power manipulation on any of these other- versus self-orientation scales, Fs(2, 100)  1.91, p  .15.3 These results show that our manipulation did not affect self- and otherorientation. 2 We also checked whether these manipulation checks mediated the main effects. This was not the case. This might have been caused by the strong effects on, and low variance of, these manipulation checks. 3 Reassuringly, these nonsignificant trends showed that social power nonsignificantly decreased altruism, and nonsignificantly increased individualism, compared with control and personal-power conditions.

4

2479

Participants and Design Readers of the national monthly magazine Intermediair, a Dutch journal aimed at mid- and high-level employees, were asked to voluntarily complete a questionnaire on the Internet. In total, 3,082 respondents completed the questionnaire (32.9% women, 67.1% men, with a mean age of 37.1 years). Respondents varied in their levels of power at work (57.8% executive staff, 23.1% lower management, 14.7% middle management, and 4.4% top management). Measures We measured social and personal power with two single items. In keeping with a definition of social power as power over others, we measured this construct by asking respondents to indicate, on a 5-point scale ranging from not at all, 1, to very much, 5, to what degree they influenced and affected other people in their organization. In keeping with a definition of personal power as power over oneself, we measured this construct by asking respondents to indicate, on a 5-point scale ranging from none, 1, to very much, 5, to what degree they had power over themselves (i.e., could do what they wanted). Respondents then completed a series of 100 unrelated items (taking approximately 8–10 min), which served as a filler task. Next, we measured stereotyping by asking respondents to indicate to what degree they thought 12 gender-stereotypic traits applied to men and women. Participants rated three negative male traits (i.e., aggressive, dominant, and blunt), three positive male traits (i.e., rational, assertive, and technical), three negative female traits (i.e., neurotic, dependent, and unstable), and three positive female traits (i.e., talkative, sensitive, and considerate), on 9-point scales, ranging from not at all, 1, to very much, 9 (a 5 .83). Next, we measured respondents’ behavioral approach orientation, by means of the Promotion Strategy Index (Sassenberg, Jonas, Shah, & Brazy, 2007). The promotion strategy index is an accepted, short measure of regulatory fit and measures behavioral approach and inhibition (promotion and prevention) on five 9-point bipolar scales (a 5 .71). Finally, we asked respondents for their gender, age, education, and length of employment, to be able to control for these.

Volume ]]]—Number ]]

Joris Lammers, Janka I. Stoker, and Diederik A. Stapel

All of our dependent and independent variables were normally distributed. Results Social and Personal Power As we had predicted, social and personal power correlated positively (r 5 .44, p < .001). This simple bivariate correlation was about equally strong for men (r 5 .45) and for women (r 5 .41). There were no indications that this correlation was nonlinear or that it was weaker or stronger at higher or lower levels of power.

(BWUS PSCI 2479 Webpdf:=10/22/2009 03:43:42 200528 Bytes 7 PAGES n operator=S.arsathali) 10/22/2009 3:43:48 PM

Stereotyping We then checked how social and personal power were related to stereotyping by performing a regression analysis. In a first step, in which we entered only demographic variables, we found a negative effect of age (b 5 0.01, p 5 .003), of education (b 5 0.07, p 5 .007), and of gender (women stereotyped less than men; b 5 0.18, p < .001). We then added in a second step personal and social power. As expected, we found that personal power increased stereotyping (b 5 0.05, 95% confidence interval, or CI 5 [0.01, 0.09], p 5 .01), whereas social power decreased stereotyping (b 5 0.04, 95% CI 5 [0.08, 0.01], p 5 .03).

PSCI

Approach When we entered the demographic variables, we found a positive effect of length of employment (b 5 0.03, p < .001), of education (b 5 0.11, p 5 .002), and of age (b 5 0.03, p < .001). We then added personal and social power. As expected, personal power (b 5 0.22, 95% CI 5 [0.19, 0.26], p < .001), and social power (b 5 0.18, 95% CI 5 [0.13, 0.24], p < .001) both increased behavioral approach tendencies. These results replicated the effects of Study 1 by measuring rather than manipulating social and personal power. Again, social power and personal power had parallel effects on approach but opposite effects on stereotyping, even though they were positively correlated. GENERAL DISCUSSION

Power has a strong influence on human behavior. It not only gives people influence over the world and other people but also has a wide range of strong side effects on cognition and behavior: People who have power think and act differently from people who lack it (Kipnis, 1972). If we want to understand society, we need to understand how power affects behavior (Russell, 1938/ 1960). In the past decades, psychologists have intensively tried to do this, generating a wealth of literature on the effect of power on cognition and behavior. The current work shows that studying power as a monolithic concept is unlikely to further our understanding of its effects. We

Volume ]]]—Number ]]

2479

distinguished personal and social power and showed that these two types of power can have different effects because they are differentially associated with interdependence and independence. Personal power increases stereotyping because it is associated with independence and freedom, but social power decreases stereotyping because it is associated with interdependence and responsibility. When the independence-interdependence distinction is irrelevant, such as for behavioral approach, personal power and social power have parallel effects. Limitations We showed these effects of personal and social power on stereotyping and approach by (a) priming (Study 1) and (b) measuring (Study 2). Some readers may wonder about the ecological validity of these effects. We note, however, that the priming manipulations and measures of power we used have similar effects as structural manipulations of power (see Anderson & Galinsky, 2006; Chen, Lee-Chai, & Bargh, 2001). Other readers may wonder about the ecological validity of our rather abstract dependent variables. We argue, however, that this is a strength rather than a weakness of our approach, as more elaborate measures are likely to be associated with related concepts, such as responsibility, and thus lead to confounded results. The abstract measures we used easily generalize across different types of power. Furthermore, previous research has shown that such abstract measures have had similar effects to more face-valid measures of approach (e.g. Lammers et al., 2008) and stereotyping (e.g. Devine, 1989). Suggestions for Future Research We found in Study 1 that the manipulation check of general power correlated more strongly with that of social power than with that of personal power. This suggests that the subjective meaning that participants give to the concept of power overlaps more with social power than with personal power. Future research might want to determine to what degree popular primes of general power (e.g. Galinsky et al., 2003, Experiment 2) manipulate social and personal power. This would allow a more detailed understanding of the nature of their effects. On a related note, our finding that general behavioral approach is unaffected by the distinction between social and personal power does not mean that more specific behavioral effects are similarly unaffected. If the distinction between independence and interdependence is relevant, then this more specific behavior will also be affected. Most likely, personal power increases behavior that goes against social norms and constraints, while social power decreases such counternormative behavior (cf. Galinsky et al., 2003, Study 3). Our results also do not imply that all cognitive effects of power are subject to the distinction between social and personal power. For example, Smith and Trope (2006) found that feelings of elevated power increase abstract thinking. This perceptual effect of power is

5

Differentiating Social and Personal Power

most likely unrelated to either independence or interdependence and is therefore invariant to the personal/social power distinction. Links to Existing Literature Our results link to other research that shows the malleability of power effects. Personal (Chen et al., 2001), cultural (Zhong, Magee, Maddux, & Galinsky, 2006), and situational (Lammers et al., 2008) associations with power can drastically affect the results. In addition, we show that even within the concept of power lies the root for such disparities. Power is too basic a phenomenon to have simple effects.

(BWUS PSCI 2479 Webpdf:=10/22/2009 03:43:42 200528 Bytes 7 PAGES n operator=S.arsathali) 10/22/2009 3:43:48 PM

REFERENCES

PSCI

Anderson, C., & Berdahl, J.L. (2002). The experience of power: Examining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1362– 1377. Anderson, C., & Galinsky, A.D. (2006). Power, optimism, and risktaking. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 511–536. Arendt, H. (1969). On violence. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World. Aron, A., Aron, E.N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596–612. Banaji, M.R., Hardin, C., & Rothman, A.J. (1993). Implicit stereotyping in person judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 272–281. Bass, B.M. (1998). Transformational leadership: Individual, military and educational impact. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Cartwright, D. (1959). A field theoretical conception of power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 183–220). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan. Chen, S., Lee-Chai, A.Y., & Bargh, J.A. (2001). Relationship orientation as moderator of the effects of social power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 183–187. Chen, S., Ybarra, O., & Kiefer, A.K. (2004). Power and impression formation: The effects of power on the desire for morality and competence information. Social Cognition, 22, 391–421. Cross, S.E., Bacon, P.L., & Morris, M.L. (2000). The relational-interdependent self-construal and relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 791–808. Davis, M.H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126. Devine, P.G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5–18. Emerson, R.M. (1962). Power dependence relations. American Journal of Sociology, 27, 31–41. Fiske, S.T. (1993). Controlling other people: The impact of power on stereotyping. American Psychologist, 48, 621–628. Fiske, S.T., & Berdahl, J.L. (2007). Social power. In A. Kruglanski & E.T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: A handbook of basic principles (pp. 678–692). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. Fiske, S.T., & De´pret, E. (1996). Control, interdependence and power: Understanding social cognition in its social context. European Review of Social Psychology, 7, 31–61.

6

2479

Fiske, S.T., & Neuberg, S.L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. In M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1–74). Amsterdam: Academic Press. French, J.R.P. Jr., & Raven, B.H. (1959). The bases of social power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 118–149). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan, Institute of Social Research. Galinsky, A.D., Gruenfeld, D.H., & Magee, J.C. (2003). From power to action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453– 466. Galinsky, A.D., Magee, J.C., Gruenfeld, D.H., Whitson, J., & Liljenquist, K.A. (2008). Social power reduces the strength of the situation: Implications for creativity, conformity, and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1450–1466. Goodwin, S.A., Gubin, A., Fiske, S.T., & Yzerbyt, V.Y. (2000). Power can bias impression processes: Stereotyping subordinates by default and by design. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 3, 227–256. Higgins, E.T., Rholes, W.S., & Jones, C.R. (1977). Category accessibility and impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 141–154. Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D.H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284. Kipnis, D. (1972). Does power corrupt? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 33–41. Lammers, J., Galinsky, A.D., Gordijn, E.H., & Otten, S. (2008). Illegitimacy moderates the effects of power on approach. Psychological Science, 19, 558–564. Neuberg, S.L., & Fiske, S.T. (1987). Motivational influences on impression formation: Outcome dependency, accuracy-driven attention, and individuating processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 431–444. Overbeck, J.R. (in press). Concepts, domains, and historical perspectives on power. In A. Guinote & T. Vescio (Eds.), The social psychology of power. New York: Guilford Press. Overbeck, J.R., & Park, B. (2001). When power does not corrupt: Superior individuation processes among powerful perceivers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 549–565. Overbeck, J.R., & Park, B. (2006). Powerful perceivers, powerless objects: Flexibility of powerholders’ social attention. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 99, 227–243. Parsons, T.E.F. (1967). Sociological theory and modern society. New York: Free Press. Ravlin, E.C., & Meglino, B.M. (1987). Effect of values on perception and decision making: A study of alternative work values measures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 666–673. Russell, B. (1960). Power, a new social analysis. London: Allen & Unwin. (Original work published 1938) Sassenberg, K., Jonas, K.J., Shah, J., & Brazy, P. (2007). Why some groups just feel better: The regulatory fit of group power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 249–267. Smith, P.K., & Bargh, J.A. (2008). Nonconscious effects of power on basic approach and avoidance tendencies. Social Cognition, 26, 1–24. Smith, P.K., & Trope, Y. (2006). You focus on the forest when you’re in charge of the trees: Power priming and abstract information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 578–596.

Volume ]]]—Number ]]

Joris Lammers, Janka I. Stoker, and Diederik A. Stapel

Stapel, D.A., & Koomen, W. (2001). When we wonder what it all means: Interpretation goals facilitate accessibility and stereotyping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 915–929. Van Dijke, M., & Poppe, M. (2006). Striving for personal power as a basis for social power dynamics. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 537–556. Van Lange, P.A.M. (1999). The pursuit of joint outcomes and equality in outcomes: An integrative model of social value orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 337–349. Weber, M. (1978). Economy and society. Berkeley: University of California Press. (Original work published 1914) Zhong, C., Magee, J.C., Maddux, W.W., & Galinsky, A.D. (2006). Power, culture, and action: Considerations in the expression and enactment of power in East Asian and Western societies. In E.A. Mannix, M.A. Neale, & Y. Chen (Eds.), Research on managing in teams and groups (Vol. 9, pp. 53–73). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

SUPPORTING INFORMATION Additional supporting information may be found in the online version of this article:

Table S1. Factor analysis of the manipulation checks of Study 1. Table S2. Results for manipulation checks of Study 1. Please note: Wiley-Blackwell is not responsible for the content or functionality of any supporting materials supplied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing material) should be directed to the corresponding author for the article.

(BWUS PSCI 2479 Webpdf:=10/22/2009 03:43:42 200528 Bytes 7 PAGES n operator=S.arsathali) 10/22/2009 3:43:48 PM

(RECEIVED 5/22/08; REVISION ACCEPTED 4/28/09)

PSCI

Volume ]]]—Number ]]

2479

7

Author Query Form _______________________________________________________ Journal Article

PSCI 2479

_______________________________________________________ Dear Author, During the copy-editing of your paper, the following queries arose. Please respond to these by marking up your proofs with the necessary changes/additions. Please write your answers clearly on the query sheet if there is insufficient space on the page proofs. If returning the proof by fax do not write too close to the paper's edge. Please remember that illegible mark-ups may delay publication. Query No. Description Author Response No Queries

.

2010 Lammers Stoker Stapel Social Personal PSCI.pdf

There was a problem loading more pages. Whoops! There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. 2010 Lammers Stoker Stapel Social Personal PSCI.pdf. 2010 Lammers Stoker Stapel Social Personal PSCI.pdf. Open. Extract.

217KB Sizes 3 Downloads 257 Views

Recommend Documents

2010 Lammers Stapel dehumanization GPIR.pdf
Page 3 of 14. 2010 Lammers Stapel dehumanization GPIR.pdf. 2010 Lammers Stapel dehumanization GPIR.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu.

2009 Lammers Stoker Stapel Power and Approach EJSP.pdf ...
2009 Lammers Stoker Stapel Power and Approach EJSP.pdf. 2009 Lammers Stoker Stapel Power and Approach EJSP.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In.

2010 Lammers Stapel Galinsky Hypocrisy.pdf
In the political domain, newspa- pers repeatedly report on government officials who have. extramarital affairs despite decrying the breakdown of family. values or ...

2010 Lammers Stapel Galinsky Hypocrisy.pdf
In the political domain, newspa- pers repeatedly report on government officials who have. extramarital affairs despite decrying the breakdown of family. values or ...

2012 Johnson and Lammers The powerful disregard social ...
2012 Johnson and Lammers The powerful disregard social comparison information.pdf. 2012 Johnson and Lammers The powerful disregard social comparison ...

2012 Inbar Lammers Political Diversity in Social and Personality ...
psychologists in each of these three domains and overall. Accuracy in Perceived .... statistics available for comparison purposes in position demographic in the ... 2012 Inbar Lammers Political Diversity in Social and Personality Psychology.pdf.

2012 Stoker Velde Lammers.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. 2012 Stoker ...

Lammers Dubois Rucker Galinsky.pdf
Feb 24, 2013 - for her: two years in a row she gave job talks at a number of top 10. schools and universities, but got no offers from those schools. Yet, in. 2009 ...

Bram Stoker - A Pele Vermelha.pdf
pico do seu movimento, o animal. caiu para trás, e ficou com um aspecto ainda mais terrível porque tombou. Page 3 of 13. Bram Stoker - A Pele Vermelha.pdf.

catawba county social services 2010 annual report
management system, which will provide more information to the ... It is important to note that many of the .... tance by providing short-term crisis assistance to eligible citizens. ..... creating a coordinated database to capture school progress and

catawba county social services 2010 annual report
management system, which will provide more information to the public about ..... School Social Workers reduce barriers that affect student academic success.

B.A.-Social-Science-2010-Paper.pdf
National Rural Employment Guarantee Act ensures employment for: (1) All persons. (2) Selected persons. (3) Those eligible and who opt for it. (4) Each member ...

2013 Lammers and Proulx Writing autobiographical narratives ...
2013 Lammers and Proulx Writing autobiographical narratives increases political conservatism JESP.pdf. 2013 Lammers and Proulx Writing autobiographical ...

bram stoker dracula download pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. bram stoker ...

2012 Lammers Abstraction increases hypocrisy.pdf
Page 1 of 6. Reports. Abstraction increases hypocrisy☆. Joris Lammers ⁎. Tilburg University, The Netherlands. article info abstract. Article history: Received 6 April 2011. Revised 10 July 2011. Available online 23 July 2011. Keywords: Morality.

B.A.-Social-Science-2010-Paper.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... B.A.-Social- ... 10-Paper.pdf. B.A.-Social-S ... 010-Paper.pdf. Open.

2012 Lammers Abstraction increases hypocrisy.pdf
Results of Experiments 1–4. Data show Means (SDs) per cell and the degree of hypocrisy (operationalized as the difference between the moral acceptability of ...

2011 Lammers Power Increases Infidelity Psychological Science.pdf ...
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. 2011 Lammers ...

2012 Lammers Galinsky Gordijn Otten.pdf
Page 1 of 10. http://spp.sagepub.com/. Social Psychological and Personality Science. http://spp.sagepub.com/content/3/3/282. The online version of this article ...

2013 Lammers Dubois Rucker Galinsky Power gets the job JESP.pdf ...
Feb 24, 2013 - or an administrative procedure) and soft skills, which refer to how. people relate to others and leverage their know-how in social contexts.

2011 Lammers Power Increases Infidelity Psychological Science.pdf ...
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item. 2011 Lammers ...

NET Dec 2010 Question Paper II Social Medicine and Community ...
Sign in. Page. 1. /. 16. Loading… ... Vaccine. 5. Typhoid fever is transmitted by .... NET Dec 2010 Question Paper II Social Medicine and Community Health.pdf.

Banaras Hindu University M.A. Social Work 2010.pdf
Banaras Hindu University M.A. Social Work 2010.pdf. Banaras Hindu University M.A. Social Work 2010.pdf. Open. Extract. Open with. Sign In. Main menu.

2010-BtoB-Social-Media-Marketing-Award-Winner.pdf
There was a problem previewing this document. Retrying... Download. Connect more apps... Try one of the apps below to open or edit this item.