Supplementary Online Material for:

Metacognitive ability predicts learning cue-stimulus associations in the absence of external feedback

Marine Hainguerlot1, Jean-Christophe Vergnaud1,*, Vincent de Gardelle2,* 1

Centre d’Economie de la Sorbonne, CNRS UMR 8174, Paris, France

2

CNRS and Paris School of Economics, Paris, France.

*

denotes equal contribution

Corresponding author: Vincent de Gardelle. Centre d’Economie de la Sorbonne, 112 boulevard de l’Hopital, 75013 Paris, France Tel: 0144078742. Email: [email protected]

This file includes the following items:   

3 Supplementary Tables (Table S1, Table S2 and Table S3) 1 Supplementary Figure (Figure S1) 1 Supplementary Analysis (‘Mathematical argument’)

1

(1) Logit (2) OLS Cue identification Final accuracy b se p b se p Metacog 1.805 0.777 0.020 0.027 0.011 0.014 W. Memory 0.133 0.059 0.023 0.001 0.001 0.172 Initial accuracy 11.812 5.645 0.036 0.643 0.076 <0.001 Calibrated dots 0.074 0.107 0.490 0.003 0.002 0.077 Order -0.034 0.590 0.954 0.013 0.009 0.152 Metacog*Order -0.002 0.305 0.996 0.006 0.004 0.176 Table S1: Multivariate regression results. (1) Logit regression with successful cue identification as a dependent variable. (2) OLS regression with final performance as a dependent variable. Explanatory variable: metacognitive efficiency. Control variables: working memory, initial accuracy, calibrated difference in the number of dots, order in which the sessions where performed (=1 if confidence session first, 0 otherwise) and interaction term between metacognitive efficiency and session order.

Response rate left

Accuracy, valid cue trials -

Accuracy, invalid cue trials -

SDT estimates c d’

0.502 0.737 0.004 1.340 (0.071) (0.068) (0.230) (0.479) Left cue 0.685 0.767 0.801 0.665 0.215 1.354 (0.076) (0.062) (0.081) (0.138) (0.287) (0.488) Right cue 0.316 0.758 0.795 0.648 -0.240 1.308 (0.093) (0.063) (0.096) (0.132) (0.361) (0.431) Neutal cue 0.492 0.703 -0.022 1.126 (0.107) (0.050) (0.333) (0.325) Left cue 0.598 0.701 0.701 0.700 -0.010 1.095 (0.095) (0.063) (0.100) (0.117) (0.291) (0.324) Right cue 0.393 0.707 0.708 0.704 -0.014 1.117 (0.081) (0.069) (0.095) (0.110) (0.246) (0.388) Table S2: Descriptive statistics on cue usage for “learners” (N=35) and “non- learners” (N=30). Response rate left, Average accuracy rate, accuracy rate conditional on valid and invalid cues, and SDT estimates decision criterion (c) and sensitivity (d’); with standard deviation reported between parentheses. « Nonlearners »

« Learners »

Neutral cue

Accuracy, All trials

2

Mean (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (11) (12) (13) (14) (15) (16) (SD) (1) Cue learn +0.54 1 +0.25 +0.27 +0.29 +0.28 +0.11 -0.05 +0.25 +0.45 +0.48 +0.58 +0.58 -0.22 +0.49 +0.28 +0.13 (0.50) * * * * * *** *** *** *** *** * (2) Metaratio +0.87 1 +0.93 +0.65 +0.12 -0.07 -0.10 -0.21 -0.02 +0.02 -0.03 -0.03 +0.01 -0.03 -0.02 -0.10 (0.42) *** *** (3) log(metaratio) -0.27 1 +0.63 +0.10 -0.04 -0.06 -0.15 +0.05 +0.09 -0.02 +0.01 +0.07 -0.04 +0.05 -0.03 (0.57) *** (4) Brier Resol. +0.01 1 +0.17 -0.09 -0.09 -0.03 +0.13 +0.16 +0.02 +0.05 +0.07 -0.01 +0.14 +0.22 (0.01) (5) W. memory +41.72 1 -0.33 +0.06 -0.08 +0.01 +0.03 +0.06 +0.02 -0.09 +0.05 -0.01 +0.09 (6.21) ** (6) Cal. Dots +9.95 1 -0.24 +0.47 +0.47 +0.45 +0.26 +0.40 +0.23 +0.14 +0.46 +0.19 (3.36) *** *** *** * *** *** (7) Order +0.54 1 -0.06 +0.00 +0.02 +0.06 +0.02 -0.12 +0.09 -0.04 -0.08 (0.50) (8) Initial perf. +0.73 1 +0.84 +0.77 +0.46 +0.67 +0.32 +0.21 +0.77 +0.11 (0.07) *** *** *** *** ** *** (9) Overall perf. +0.73 1 +0.99 +0.59 +0.82 +0.33 +0.30 +0.92 +0.27 (0.06) *** *** *** ** * *** * (10) Final perf. +0.73 1 +0.60 +0.82 +0.31 +0.31 +0.92 +0.29 (0.06) *** *** * * *** * (11) Cong. Rate +0.65 1 +0.94 -0.54 +0.93 +0.30 +0.10 (0.07) *** *** *** * (12) Acc. : valid +0.75 1 -0.21 +0.75 +0.59 +0.16 (0.08) *** *** (13) Acc. : invalid +0.68 1 -0.79 +0.58 +0.11 (0.10) *** *** (14) Criterion bias +0.25 1 -0.00 +0.06 (0.46) (15) d’ : cue sess +1.23 1 +0.36 (0.39) ** (16) d’ : conf. sess +1.21 1 (0.43) Table S3: Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations for all the variables. N= 65 participants. p-values: *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001 The variables are: (1) Successful identification of the cues after the cueing session. (2) Ratio of Meta-d’ over d’. (3) Logarithm of this ratio. (4) resolution index from the Brier score. (5) average score in the working memory tests. (6) Calibrated difference in number of dots in the cueing session. (7) Order of the cueing and confidence session. (8) Initial performance in the cueing session. (9) Overall performance in the cueing session. (10) Final performance in the cueing session. (11) Rate of responses congruent with the predictive cue. (12) Accuracy in the valid cue trials. (13) Accuracy in the invalid cue trials. (14) Difference in the decision criteria for the left and right predictive cues. (15) d’ in the cueing session. (16) d’ in the confidence session.

3

1

0.5

0 L

1

0 L

ME=0.50

d'=1.28

d'=0.91

d'=0.71

d'=0.95

d'=1.11

d'=1.08

d'=1.26

d'=1.12

d'=2.26

d'=1.25

N

0.5

R

0 L

N

0.5

R

1 d'=1.06

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

0 L

R

d'=1.34

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

1

0 L

R

d'=2.00

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

1

0 L

R

d'=1.09

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

1

0 L

R

d'=1.27

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

1

0 L

R

d'=1.65

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

1

0 L

R

d'=1.00

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

1

0 L

R

d'=1.07

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

1

0 L

d'=1.41

N

0.5

R

0 L

N

0.5

R

1

0 L

N

ME=0.91

ME=0.91

ME=0.92

ME=0.95

ME=0.96

ME=0.99

ME=1.00

d'=0.93

d'=1.21

d'=1.64

d'=0.93

d'=1.03

d'=1.59

d'=1.49

N

R

0 L

N

R

1

0.5

R

0 L

d'=1.06

N

0.5

R

N

0 L

R

0 L

0.5

R

0 L

N

R

d'=0.89

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

0 L

R

d'=0.79

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

0 L

R

d'=2.59

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

0 L

R

d'=1.08

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

0 L

R

d'=0.83

N

0.5

R

0 L

0.5

R

0 L

R

d'=1.10

N

0.5

R

0 L

d'=0.88

N

0.5

R

0 L

0 L

0.5

R

0 L

N

R

d'=0.84

N

R

N

ME=1.02 0.5

R

0 L

d'=1.00

N

R

1 ME=1.25

ME=1.29

d'=1.30

N

0.5

R

0 L

d'=1.20

N

R

1 ME=1.79

d'=1.30

R

1

1

N

0 L

ME=1.22

ME=1.71

d'=0.81

N

0.5

1

1

N

0 L

ME=1.21

ME=1.56

d'=1.02

N

0.5

1

1

N

0 L

ME=1.18

ME=1.53

d'=0.92

N

1

1

N

0 L

ME=1.12

ME=1.51

d'=1.23

N

1

1

N

0 L

ME=1.10

ME=1.42

d'=1.11

N

1

1

N

0 L

ME=1.08

ME=1.34

d'=1.21

N

0 L 1

1 ME=1.33

0.5

R

ME=1.07

1 d'=1.20

N

1 ME=1.06

d'=1.44

N

0 L

0.5

d'=1.51

1

d'=1.15

0.5

0 L

0.5

1

ME=0.86

0.5

R

ME=0.79

d'=1.24

d'=0.98

0.5

R

ME=0.78

d'=0.92

N

1

ME=0.86

0.5

0.5

1

N

0 L

ME=0.67

d'=1.29

d'=1.50

0.5

R

d'=1.19

1

ME=0.83 0.5

0.5

ME=0.67

ME=0.78

d'=0.77

N

ME=0.51

1

1

N

0 L

ME=0.64

ME=0.77

d'=0.95

N

0.5

1

1

N

0 L

ME=0.60

ME=0.76

d'=1.13

N

0.5

1

1

N

0 L

ME=0.59

ME=0.72

d'=0.95

N

0.5

1

1

N

0 L

ME=0.59

ME=0.72

d'=3.03

N

0.5

1

1

N

0 L

ME=0.58

ME=0.71

d'=0.22

N

0.5

1

1

N

0 L

ME=0.58

ME=0.70

d'=1.63

N

0.5

1

1

N

0 L

ME=0.56

ME=0.69

d'=1.04

N

0.5

1

1

N

0 L

ME=0.56

ME=1.32

0 L

1

ME=0.42

1

0.5

1

ME=0.40

ME=1.05

0 L

1

ME=0.38

1

0.5

1

ME=0.26

1

0.5

1

ME=0.25

ME=0.69

0 L

1

ME=0.23

1

0.5

1

ME=0.19

ME=0.53

0 L

1

ME=0.19

1

0.5

1

ME=0.14

ME=2.32

d'=1.55

N

0.5

R

0 L

d'=0.66

N

R

Figure S1. Individual data for the confidence session. For each individual, the figure indicates the metacognitive efficiency (ME), the perceptual d’ and plots the distributions of response x confidence ratings (from high confidence left to high confidence right response) for the left stimulus (in blue) and the right stimulus (in green). 4

SUPPLEMENTARY ANALYSES Mathematical argument We propose here a formalization of our hypothesis linking metacognition to cue identification. We claim that the metacognitive ability of a participant, that is, her ability to assign a lower confidence to her errors, is positively related to her ability to identify the predictive values of the cues. This claim rests on the assumption that both metacognition and learning rely on how well the participant can process the information provided by the stimuli. On a given trial, after receiving the stimulus (𝑆 = 𝑅 𝑜𝑟 𝐿) the observer would form a belief about the stimulus being R, noted 𝑃(𝑆 = 𝑅), or L, noted 𝑃(𝑆 = 𝐿). This belief will be the basis for the observer’s decision and confidence judgment: when 𝑃(𝑆 = 𝑅) > 𝑃(𝑆 = 𝐿), the observer selects the response 𝑅 and her confidence is equal to 𝑃(𝑆 = 𝑅), and when 𝑃(𝑆 = 𝑅) < 𝑃(𝑆 = 𝐿), she reports 𝐿 with confidence 𝑃(𝑆 = 𝐿). In a cue learning situation, this belief would be the basis for identifying the predictive value of the cue, that is, the probability 𝜋 of the stimulus 𝑅 when the cue is present. The observer can form an estimate 𝜋̂ of this value by considering her average belief about the presence of the stimulus 𝑅 in these trials. To formally relate 𝜋̂ to the metacognitive ability of the observer, let us examine how it depends on ̅̅̅ the observer's average confidence when she is correct (𝐶 𝐶 ) and her average confidence when she is incorrect (𝐶̅𝐼 ). As there are two types of stimuli and two types of responses, there will be 4 types of trials, described in the table below. Here, we also assume that the subject's accuracy rate 𝐴 and confidence when correct or incorrect do not depend on the stimulus being 𝑅 or 𝐿, for sake of simplicity. Stimulus presented (with probability p) R, with p= 𝜋 L, with p= 1 − 𝜋

Response made (with probability p) R, with p= 𝐴 L, with p= 1 − 𝐴 L, with p= 𝐴 R, with p= 1 − 𝐴

Confidence in the response ̅̅̅ 𝐶 𝐶 𝐶̅𝐼 ̅̅̅ 𝐶 𝐶 𝐶̅𝐼

Belief about R being present ̅̅̅ 𝐶𝐶 1 − 𝐶̅𝐼 ̅̅̅ 1−𝐶 𝐶 ̅ 𝐶𝐼

Across these 4 cases, we can define the net belief that the stimulus 𝑅 was presented, that is, the estimate 𝜋̂ made by the observer. ̅̅̅ ̅ ̅̅̅ ̅ 𝜋̂ = 𝜋𝐴𝐶 𝐶 + 𝜋(1 − 𝐴)(1 − 𝐶𝐼 ) + (1 − 𝜋)𝐴(1 − 𝐶𝐶 ) + (1 − 𝜋)(1 − 𝐴)𝐶𝐼 Furthermore, if we assume that the observer’s mean confidence is equal to her accuracy rate 𝐴, we ̅̅̅ ̅ can produce a relation between 𝜋̂, 𝐴 and the difference between 𝐶 𝐶 and 𝐶𝐼 , which we will denote by 𝑟 for the resolution of confidence with respect to errors. This difference quantifies metacognitive ability in a simplistic manner but it is appropriate for the present illustration. ̅̅̅ ̅ The assumption that the average confidence is 𝐴 across all trials gives us: 𝐴𝐶 𝐶 + (1 − 𝐴)𝐶𝐼 = 𝐴 5

By definition we also have 𝑟 = ̅̅̅ 𝐶𝐶 − 𝐶̅𝐼 ̅̅̅ ̅ Combining these two equations, we find that 𝐶 𝐶 = 𝐴 − 𝐴𝑟 + 𝑟 and that 𝐶𝐼 = 𝐴 − 𝐴𝑟 ̅̅̅ ̅ Note that the extreme case of perfect error detection would correspond to 𝑟 = 1, 𝐶 𝐶 = 1, 𝐶𝐼 = 0 and ̅̅̅ ̅ that the extreme case of null resolution would correspond to 𝑟 = 0, 𝐶 𝐶 = 𝐴, 𝐶𝐼 = 𝐴 Introducing these expressions in our calculation of 𝜋̂ yields, after simplification: 𝜋̂ = 𝜋 − 2𝐴(2𝜋 − 1)(1 − 𝐴)(1 − 𝑟) Thus, for any given 𝜋 greater than 0.5, and assuming that both 𝐴 and 𝑟 are between 0 and 1, this expression tells us that 𝜋̂ should always fall below 𝜋, and will get closer to 𝜋 when accuracy gets closer to 1 or when resolution gets closer to 1. The figure below illustrates how 𝜋̂ depends on A and r, for a target value of 𝜋 = 0.75. We clearly see that the estimated probability of the cue-stimulus association approaches the target value of .75 (in red) when accuracy and confidence resolution increases. In other words, both perceptual ability and metacognitive ability improve cue identification.

1

0.75

0.9

0.7

0.85

Accuracy

0.8

0.65

0.75 0.7

0.6

0.65 0.6

0.55

Estimated cue-stimulus association

0.95

0.55 0.5

0

0.2

0.4 0.6 Confidence resolution

0.8

1

0.5

6

1 Supplementary Online Material for: Metacognitive ...

Control variables: working memory, initial accuracy, calibrated difference in the number of dots, order in which the sessions where performed (=1 if confidence.

692KB Sizes 0 Downloads 187 Views

Recommend Documents

Supplementary Online Material
Branstetter, M.G. (2009) The ant genus Stenamma Westwood (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) redefined, with a description of a new genus Propodilobus. Zootaxa,.

Supplementary Material
Jan Heufer ∗. *TU Dortmund University, Department of Economics and Social Science, 44221 Dortmund,. Germany. .... 3 More Details on the Data Analysis.

Supplementary Material for
Fujitsu Research & Development Center Co., Ltd, Beijing, China. {wangzhengxiang,rjliu}@cn.fujitsu.com. Abstract. This supplementary material provides the ...

Supplementary Material for
Aug 3, 2016 - alternatives are “autocracy and risk-sharing” and “democracy and investment.” In other words, the .... whether seizing power. A coup is .... Our main sources are Galasso (1976), Stearns (2001), and Ortu (2005). References.

Supplementary Material for
Sep 25, 2015 - Using archived and field-collected worker bumble bees, we tested for temporal changes in the tongue ... (Niwot Ridge Long Term Ecological Research Site, 40°3.567'N, 105°37.000'W), and by. P. A. Byron (17) ... these transects did not

1 Supplementary online material: additional results ...
weakly higher winning frequencies for A and C in comparison to D. The corresponding tests reject the .... tives over the course of the experiment for PV elections.

Supplementary Material
gaze fixation point is detected by an eye tracker. ∗indicates equal contribution. (a) Random ... 1) confirms quanti- tatively our intuition, that the location of the hands are in- deed important for learning about hand-object interactions. Based on

Supplementary Material - Arkivoc
General Papers. ARKIVOC 2015 (vii) S1-S13. Page S3. ©ARKAT-USA, Inc. δ /ppm. 1. H Assignment. 8.60 (brs, 1H). 5.35 (t, J = 7.3 Hz, 1H). 5.08 (t, J = 6.1 Hz, ...

Supplementary Material
By the definition of conjunctive pattern, since S does not satisfy pc, then S ... Then there exists at least one instance of p o in S: {I1,I2,...,Im},. I1 ∈ I(p c. 1 ,S),...,Im ∈ I(p c m,S), such that ∀t1 ∈. I1,..., ∀tm ∈ Im,t1 < t2 < ...

Supplementary Material
and Business Cycles Conference, the Bank of Canada 2014 Fellowship ..... CGH exhibited an upward trend in the frequency of sales that could explain why they find ...... 12Fox and Sayed (2016) use the IRI scanner dataset to document the ...

Supplementary Material
The data are provided a for 10 subsectors, which I aggregate up to three sectors as follows. ... CAN Canada ...... Structural change in an open economy. Journal ...

Supplementary Material
SVK Slovakia. CYP Cyprus. ITA. Italy. SVN Slovenia. CZE Czech Republic JPN Japan. SWE Sweden. DEU Germany. KOR South Korea. TUR Turkey. DNK Denmark. LTU Lithuania. TWN Taiwan. ESP Spain. LUX Luxembourg. USA United States. EST Estonia. LVA Latvia. RoW

Supplementary Material for ``Observations on ...
Nov 19, 2017 - C.2 Steady State in a Perturbed Environment. In this subsection we formally adapt the definitions of a consistent signal profile and of a steady state to perturbed environments. Let f((1−ϵ)·σ+ϵ·λ) : OS → OS be the mapping bet

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL FOR “WEAK MONOTONICITY ...
This representation is convenient for domains with complete orders. 1 .... check dominant-strategy implementability of many classical social choice rules. In.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL FOR “WEAK MONOTONICITY ...
This representation is convenient for domains with complete orders. 1 ... v = (0,v2,0), v2 > 0, would want to deviate and misreport their type so as to get 3.

Efficient Repeated Implementation: Supplementary Material
strategy bi except that at (ht,θt) it reports z + 1. Note from the definition of mechanism g∗ and the transition rules of R∗ that such a deviation at (ht,θt) does not ...

Electronic supplementary material
Jun 22, 2009 - ... of two Hill functions (a-b) with sufficiently distant midpoints is equivalent to the Hill function with the largest midpoint (c). Namely: Hill(x, K 1) · Hill(x, K 2) ≈ Hill(x, K 2) where K 1

Supplementary Material - HEC Montréal
... the ONS website: http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/datasets-and-tables/index.html. .... sitivity of sales to the unemployment rate for independent stores and chains.

Supplementary Material for ”Production-Based Measures of Risk for ...
Measures of Risk for Asset Pricing”. Frederico Belo. ∗. November 3, 2009. Appendix A Producer's Maximization Problem. Define the vector of state variables as xit-1 = (Kit-1,ϵit-1,Pit-1,Zit-1), where Kit-1 is the current period stock of capital,

Supplementary Material for ”Production-Based Measures of Risk for ...
Nov 3, 2009 - [4] Campbell, John Y., and Robert J. Shiller, 1988, Stock prices, earnings, and expected dividends, Journal of Finance 43,661 − 676. [5] Campbell, J., 2003, Consumption-Based Asset Pricing, in George Constantinides, Milton. Harris, an

Supplementary Materials 1. Supplementary Tables
3.87. 27. -75. -48. Orbitofrontal Cortex. 11. R. 76. 3.72. 30. 48. -15. Decreased DMN (Controls > FTD). Dorsomedial PFC. 32. L. 312. 3.98. -27. 51. 21. Anterior Cingulate. 24. -. 107. 3.83. -3. 21. 33 .... correlation pattern, corresponding to a Type

Supplementary material for “Complementary inputs and ...
Aug 2, 2017 - Figure S1: The network structures produced in the proof of Theorem 3. Undirected ...... without loss of generality that has full support on .

Supplementary Material for Adaptive Relaxed ... - CVF Open Access
1Department of Computer Science, University of Maryland, College Park, MD ... 3Department of Mathematics, Hong Kong Baptist University, Kowloon Tong, ...

Supplementary material for “Complementary inputs and ...
Aug 2, 2017 - statement of Theorem 2, and then complete the proof. ...... Jones, 1984), spaces of square-integrable functions (Harrison and Kreps, 1979; ...